Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Paolo Viviani is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Paolo Viviani.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 1992

Biological movements look uniform: evidence of motor-perceptual interactions.

Paolo Viviani; Natale Stucchi

Six experiments demonstrate a visual dynamic illusion. Previous work has shown that in 2-dimensional (2D) drawing movements, tangential velocity and radius of curvature covary in a constrained manner. The velocity of point stimuli is perceived as uniform if and only if this biological constraint is satisfied. The illusion is conspicuous: The variations of velocity in the stimuli exceed 200%. Yet movements are perceived as uniform. Conversely, 2D stimuli moving at constant velocity are perceived as strongly nonuniform. The illusion is robust: Exposure to true constant velocity fails to suppress it. Results cannot be explained entirely by the kinetic depth effect. The illusion is evidence of a coupling between motor and perceptual processes: Even in the absence of any intention to perform a movement, certain properties of the motor system implicitly influence perceptual interpretation of the visual stimulus.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 1991

A Developmental Study of the Relationship Between Geometry and Kinematics in Drawing Movements

Paolo Viviani; Roland Schneider

Trajectory and kinematics of drawing movements are mutually constrained by functional relationships that reduce the degrees of freedom of the hand-arm system. Previous investigations of these relationships are extended here by considering their development in children between 5 and 12 years of age. Performances in a simple motor task--the continuous tracing of elliptic trajectories--demonstrate that both the phenomenon of isochrony (increase of the average movement velocity with the linear extent of the trajectory) and the so-called two-thirds power law (relation between tangential velocity and curvature) are qualitatively present already at the age of 5. The quantitative aspects of these regularities evolve with age, however, and steady-state adult performance is not attained even by the oldest children. The power-law formalism developed in previous reports is generalized to encompass these developmental aspects of the control of movement.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 1985

Segmentation and coupling in complex movements.

Paolo Viviani; Marco Cenzato

In two experiments we explore the structure of complex sequences of drawing movements. We find that in these movements a single parameter--the velocity gain factor--relates the geometrical and kinematic aspects of the movement trajectory via a two-thirds power law. In Experiment 1 we investigate the relation between the velocity gain factor and the linear extent of the trajectory. In Experiment 2 we demonstrate that the gain factor provides a criterion for segmenting the movement into distinct units of motor action, and we investigate the effects of the speed of execution on this segmentation. A theoretical analysis shows that the results of both Experiments 1 and 2 can be given a unitary interpretation by assuming a coupling function of variable strength between segments. The general problem of representing motor programs is discussed within this theoretical framework.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 1998

Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing.

Philippe Vindras; Paolo Viviani

Three hypotheses concerning the control variables in visuomanual pointing were tested. Participants pointed to a visual target presented briefly in total darkness on the horizontal plane. The starting position of the hand alternated randomly among 4 points arranged as a diamond. Results show that during the experiment, movement drifted from hypometric to hypermetric. Final positions depended on the starting position. Their average pattern reproduced the diamond of the starting points, either in same orientation (hypometric trials), or with a double inversion (hypermetric trials). The distribution of variable errors was elliptical, with the major axis aligned with the direction of the movement. Statistical analysis and Monte Carlo simulations showed that the results are incompatible with the final point control hypothesis (A. Polit & E. Bizzi, 1979). Better, but not fully satisfactory, agreement was found with the view that pointing involves comparing initial and desired postures (J. F. Soechting & M. Flanders, 1989a). The hypothesis that accounted best for the results is that final hand position is coded as a vector represented in an extrinsic frame of reference centered on the hand.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 1995

Minimum-jerk, two-thirds power law, and isochrony: converging approaches to movement planning

Paolo Viviani; Tamar Flash

Two approaches to the study of movement planning were contrasted. Data on the drawing of complex two-dimensional trajectories were used to test whether the covariations of the kinematic and geometrical parameters of the movement formalized by the two-thirds power law and by the isochrony principle (P. Viviani & R. Schneider, 1991) can be derived from the minimum-jerk model hypothesis (T. Flash & N. Hogan, 1985). The convergence of the 2 approaches was satisfactory insofar as the relation between tangential velocity and curvature is concerned (two-thirds power law). Global isochrony could not be deduced from the optimal control hypothesis. Scaling of velocity within movement subunits can instead be derived from the minimum-jerk hypothesis. The implications vis-à-vis the issue of movement planning are discussed with an emphasis on the representation used by the motor control system for coding the intended trajectories.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 1989

The effect of movement velocity on form perception: Geometric illusions in dynamic displays

Paolo Viviani; Natale Stucchi

We investigated the effects of movement velocity on the perception of simple geometric trajectories. We show that when an ellipse is traced by the continuous displacement of a spot against an empty background, the subjective aspect ratio (R = vertical axis/horizontal axis) of the figure depends on the law of motion of the spot. If the tangential velocity of the spot is constant, very large and subject-specific biases emerge in the perception of the aspect ratio. If the tangential velocity of the spot is made equal to that of an elliptic motion with aspect ratioR < 1, and resulting from the vectorial composition of two harmonic functions (Lissajous motion), there is a general trend to perceive the ellipse as being flatter than in reality. The effect, however, is not symmetric: when the velocity follows a Lissajous modulation withR > 1, highly significant biases are still present in most subjects, but no common trend emerges from the experimental population. The results are discussed in the context of recent findings on the relationship between form and kinematics in spontaneous human movements.


Experimental Brain Research | 1998

Hemispheric asymmetries and bimanual asynchrony in left- and right-handers

Paolo Viviani; Daniela Perani; Franco Grassi; V. Bettinardi; Ferruccio Fazio

Abstract It is known that, when both forearms are rotated rhythmically and symmetrically, the dominant hand leads in time by about 25 ms, irrespective of movement speed. Positron emission tomography was used to test the hypothesis that the asynchrony results from a functional hemispheric asymmetry. We found that in normal, adult right-handers portions of the motor and premotor motor areas are more active in the left than in the right hemisphere. The converse pattern was observed in left-handers. The results suggest that at least some components of the neural processing involved in bimanual coordination are carried out only in the hemisphere contralateral to the dominant hand. In particular, between-hands asynchrony may reflect the time for dispatching pace-setting commands to the contralateral hemisphere.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 1997

Perceiving and tracking kinesthetic stimuli: further evidence of motor-perceptual interactions.

Paolo Viviani; Gabriel Baud-Bovy; Marco Redolfi

Two experiments pursued previous studies (P. Viviani & P. Mounoud, 1990; P. Viviani & N. Stucchi, 1989) on motor-perceptual interactions. The right arm of blindfolded participants was moved passively along elliptic trajectories. Kinematics was either coherent or at variance with the relation (two-thirds power law) observed in active movements. In Experiment 1 participants compared the horizontal and vertical extent of the ellipses. Kinematics affected aspect ratio discrimination: The direction along which the movement decelerated was subjectively stretched. In Experiment 2 participants used the left arm to reproduce in real time the movement of the right arm. The trajectories of the left arm presented a stretch similar to the perceptual illusion demonstrated in Experiment 1. Between-arm asynchrony suggests that the motor control system cannot use kinesthetic information that is at variance with the flow of reafferences normally associated with voluntary movements. It is argued that these interactions occur at the level of a central amodal representation of the stimuli.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 2000

Perceptual anticipation in handwriting: The role of implicit motor competence

Sonia Kandel; Jean-Pierre Orliaguet; Paolo Viviani

In two experiments, perceptual anticipation—that is, the observer’s ability to predict the course of dynamic visual events—in the case of handwriting traces was investigated. Observers were shown the dynamic display of the middle letter I excerpted from two cursive trigrams (lll orlln) handwritten by one individual. The experimental factor was the distribution of the velocity along the trace, which was controlled by a single parameter, γ. Only for one value of this parameter (γ=2/3) did the display comply with the two-thirds power law, which describes how tangential velocity depends on curvature in writing movements. The task was to indicate the trigram from which the trace was excerpted—that is, to guess the letter that followed the specific instance of thel that had been displayed. In Experiment 1, the no answer option was available. Experiment 2 adopted a forced-choice response rule. Responses were never reinforced. When γ=2/3, the rate of correct guesses was high (Experiment 1, P(correct)=.69; Experiment 2, P(correct)=.78). The probability of a correct answer decreased significantly for both smaller and larger values of γ, with wrong answers becoming predominant at the extremes of the range of variation of this parameter. The results are consistent with the hypothesis that perceptual anticipation of human movements involves comparing the perceptual stimulus with an internal dynamic representation of the ongoing event.


Experimental Brain Research | 2000

Proprioception does not quickly drift during visual occlusion.

Michel Desmurget; Philippe Vindras; Hélène Gréa; Paolo Viviani; Scott T. Grafton

Abstract. Several perceptual studies have shown that the ability to estimate the location of the arm degrades quickly during visual occlusion. To account for this effect, it has been suggested that proprioception drifts when not continuously calibrated by vision. In the present study, we re-evaluated this hypothesis by isolating the proprioceptive component of position sense (i.e., the subjects were forced to rely exclusively on proprioception to locate their hand, which was not the case in earlier studies). Three experiments were conducted. In experiment 1, subjects were required to estimate the location of their unseen right hand, at rest, using a visual spot controlled by the left hand through a joystick. Results showed that the mean accuracy was identical whether the localization task was performed immediately after the positioning of the hand or after a 10-s delay. In experiments 2 and 3, subjects were required to point, without vision of their limb, to visual targets. These two experiments relied on the demonstration that biases in the perception of the initial hand location induced systematic variations of the movement characteristics (initial direction, final accuracy, end-point variability). For these motor tasks, the subjects did not pay attention to the initial hand location, which removed the possible occurrence of confounding cognitive strategies. Results indicated that movement characteristics were, on average, not affected when a 15-s or 20-s delay was introduced between the positioning of the arm at the starting point and the presentation of the target. When considered together, our results suggest that proprioception does not quickly drift in the absence of visual information. The potential origin of the discrepancy between our results and earlier studies is discussed.

Collaboration


Dive into the Paolo Viviani's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Francesco Lacquaniti

University of Rome Tor Vergata

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Michel Desmurget

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge