Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Patrick Bonin is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Patrick Bonin.


Behavior Research Methods Instruments & Computers | 2003

A new set of 299 pictures for psycholinguistic studies: French norms for name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, age of acquisition, and naming latencies

Patrick Bonin; Ronald Peereman; Nathalie Malardier; Alain Méot; Marylène Chalard

Pictures are often used as stimuli in studies of perception, language, and memory. Since performances on different sets of pictures are generally contrasted, stimulus selection requires the use of standardized material to match pictures across different variables. Unfortunately, the number of standardized pictures available for empirical research is rather limited. The aim of the present study is to provide French normative data for a new set of 299 black-and-white drawings. Alario and Ferrand (1999) were closely followed in that the pictures were standardized on six variables: name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, and age of acquisition. Objective frequency measures are also provided for the most common names associated with the pictures. Comparative analyses between our results and the norms obtained in other, similar studies are reported. Finally, naming latencies corresponding to the set of pictures were also collected from French native speakers, and correlational/multiple-regression analyses were performed on naming latencies. This new set of standardized pictures is available on the Internet (http://leadserv.u-bourgogne.fr/bases/pictures/) and should be of great use to researchers when they select pictorial stimuli.


British Journal of Psychology | 2002

The determinants of spoken and written picture naming latencies.

Patrick Bonin; Marylène Chalard; Alain Méot; Michel Fayol

The influence of nine variables on the latencies to write down or to speak aloud the names of pictures taken from Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980) was investigated in French adults. The major determinants of both written and spoken picture naming latencies were image variability, image agreement and age of acquisition. To a lesser extent, name agreement was also found to have an impact in both production modes. The implications of the findings for theoretical views of both spoken and written picture naming are discussed.


Behavior Research Methods | 2010

The French Lexicon Project: Lexical decision data for 38,840 French words and 38,840 pseudowords

Ludovic Ferrand; Boris New; Marc Brysbaert; Emmanuel Keuleers; Patrick Bonin; Alain Méot; Maria Augustinova; Christophe Pallier

The French Lexicon Project involved the collection of lexical decision data for 38,840 French words and the same number of nonwords. It was directly inspired by the English Lexicon Project (Balota et al., 2007) and produced very comparable frequency and word length effects. The present article describes the methods used to collect the data, reports analyses on the word frequency and the word length effects, and describes the Excel files that make the data freely available for research purposes. The word and pseudoword data from this article may be downloaded from http://brm.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental.


Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology | 2001

Age of acquisition and word frequency in written picture naming

Patrick Bonin; Michel Fayol; Marylène Chalard

This study investigates age of acquisition (AoA) and word frequency effects in both spoken and written picture naming. In the first two experiments, reliable AoA effects on object naming speed, with objective word frequency controlled for, were found in both spoken (Experiment 1) and written picture naming (Experiment 2). In contrast, no reliable objective word frequency effects were observed on naming speed, with AoA controlled for, in either spoken (Experiment 3) or written (Experiment 4) picture naming. The implications of the findings for written picture naming are briefly discussed.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2006

Written spelling to dictation: Sound-to-spelling regularity affects both writing latencies and durations.

Marie Delattre; Patrick Bonin; Christopher Barry

The authors examined the effect of sound-to-spelling regularity on written spelling latencies and writing durations in a dictation task in which participants had to write each target word 3 times in succession. The authors found that irregular words (i.e., those containing low-probability phoneme-to-grapheme mappings) were slower both to initially produce and to execute in writing than were regular words. The regularity effect was found both when participants could and could not see their writing (Experiments 1 and 2) and was larger for low- than for high-frequency words (Experiment 3). These results suggest that central processing of the conflict generated by lexically specific and assembled spelling information for irregular words is not entirely resolved when the more peripheral processes controlling handwriting begin.


Cognition & Emotion | 2004

A prototype analysis of the French category “émotion”

Paula M. Niedenthal; Catherine Auxiette; Armelle Nugier; Nathalie Dalle; Patrick Bonin; Michel Fayol

This paper reports a prototype analysis of the French emotion lexicon, which largely replicates a previous study by Zammuner (1998) of the Italian emotion lexicon. Three measures of prototypicality were assessed, from which an explicit and an implicit indicator were computed. Prototypicality was predicted by aspects of the subjective state denoted by the word (valence, intensity, duration, familiarity) as well as characteristics of the word (objective and subjective frequency in the language, age of acquisition). Results showed that intensity was a more important predictor of prototypicality than was valence, particularly for the explicit measure of prototypicality, which was likely to be more influenced by folk theory. In addition, the predictors of the implicit and explicit measures were somewhat different. The results are discussed in the light of the distinction between émotion and sentiment in the French language. The importance of recent models of & concepts for understanding the semantics of emotion are also considered.


Behavior Research Methods | 2008

Age-of-acquisition and subjective frequency estimates for all generally known monosyllabic French words and their relation with other psycholinguistic variables

Ludovic Ferrand; Patrick Bonin; Alain Méot; Maria Augustinova; Boris New; Christophe Pallier; Marc Brysbaert

Ratings for age of acquisition (AoA) and subjective frequency were collected for the 1,493 monosyllabic French words that were most known to French students. AoA ratings were collected by asking participants to estimate in years the age at which they learned each word. Subjective frequency ratings were collected on a 7-point scale, ranging from never encountered to encountered several times daily. The results were analyzed to address the relationship between AoA and subjective frequency ratings with other psycholinguistic variables (objective frequency, imageability, number of letters, and number of orthographic neighbors). The results showed high reliability ratings with other databases. Supplementary materials for this study may be downloaded from the Psychonomic Society’s Archive of Norms, Stimuli, and Data, www.psychonomic.org/archive.


Memory & Cognition | 2000

Writing words from pictures: What representations are activated, and when?

Patrick Bonin; Michel Fayol

In three experiments, the nature of the representations involved in written picture naming and the time course of their activation were investigated. French participants had to produce picture names while hearing distractors. In Experiment 1, distractors semantically related to the picture names yielded a semantic interference effect when a stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) of − 150 msec, but not when a SOA of 0 msec, was used, in both spoken and written picture naming. Experiment 2 showed that the semantic interference effect was not located at the conceptual level. In Experiment 3, participants wrote down picture names while hearing semantically related, phonologically related, both semantically and phonologically related, or unrelated distractors, presented at both SOAs. A semantic interference effect was obtained with phonologically unrelated distractors but was eliminated with phonologically related distractors. Facilitatory effects of phonologically related distractors were found at both SOAs. The implications of the findings for written picture naming are discussed.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2011

Comparing word processing times in naming, lexical decision, and progressive demasking: evidence from Chronolex

Ludovic Ferrand; Marc Brysbaert; Emmanuel Keuleers; Boris New; Patrick Bonin; Alain Méot; Maria Augustinova; Christophe Pallier

We report performance measures for lexical decision (LD), word naming (NMG), and progressive demasking (PDM) for a large sample of monosyllabic monomorphemic French words (N = 1,482). We compare the tasks and also examine the impact of word length, word frequency, initial phoneme, orthographic and phonological distance to neighbors, age-of-acquisition, and subjective frequency. Our results show that objective word frequency is by far the most important variable to predict reaction times in LD. For word naming, it is the first phoneme. PDM was more influenced by a semantic variable (word imageability) than LD, but was also affected to a much greater extent by perceptual variables (word length, first phoneme/letters). This may reduce its usefulness as a psycholinguistic word recognition task.


Behavior Research Methods | 2011

Russian norms for name agreement, image agreement for the colorized version of the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures and age of acquisition, conceptual familiarity, and imageability scores for modal object names

Diana Tsaparina; Patrick Bonin; Alain Méot

The aim of the present study was to provide Russian normative data for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart (Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 516–536, 1980) colorized pictures (Rossion & Pourtois, Perception, 33, 217–236, 2004). The pictures were standardized on name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, imageability, and age of acquisition. Objective word frequency and objective visual complexity measures are also provided for the most common names associated with the pictures. Comparative analyses between our results and the norms obtained in other, similar studies are reported. The Russian norms may be downloaded from the Psychonomic Society supplemental archive.

Collaboration


Dive into the Patrick Bonin's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Alain Méot

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Michel Fayol

University of Luxembourg

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ludovic Ferrand

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Martial Mermillod

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Margaux Gelin

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Sébastien Roux

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge