Patrick E. Campbell
Wright State University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Patrick E. Campbell.
Bulletin of the psychonomic society | 1980
Brian M. Kruger; Patrick E. Campbell
Self-punitive behavior was demonstrated in prepunishment speeds during extinction following shock escape training in a straight runway under buzzer-CS and tone-CS conditions. Relative to the tone, the buzzer enhanced punished running (shock in the last half of the runway) and nonpunished running. These results extend the findings of Myers (1962), who reported better avoidance conditioning with a buzzer than with a tone. Self-punitive behavior, defined as the difference between punishment conditions with faster running under punishment conditions, was not significantly enhanced by the buzzer. These results do not support the suggestion (Delude, 1973) that self-punitive behavior may be uniquely affected by the use of a buzzer CS.
Bulletin of the psychonomic society | 1979
Patrick E. Campbell; Brian M. Kruger
Forty rats were given 42 acquisition and 15 extinction trials in a runway at 1 trial/day. The magnitude of water reward during acquisition was varied across five levels for independent groups (.1, 1.1, 2.1, 3.1, and 4.1 ml). Acquisition response speeds were an inverted-U-shaped function of the magnitude of reward. The results of the extinction phase showed that the rats that received the smallest reward during training showed the greatest persistence during extinction. The extinction performance of the rats that received the larger rewards during training did not differ. The results of both the acquisition and extinction phases were seen as generally consistent with the literature on the magnitude of food reward.
Bulletin of the psychonomic society | 1977
Patrick E. Campbell; Thomas A. Hinson; Brian M. Kruger
Sixty rats were trained in a runway for 42 days of acquisition and 15 days of extinction at the rate of one trial a day. Half of the animals received a 50% partial reinforcement (PR) schedule and half received a 100% reinforcement schedule (CRF). For one-third of the rats within each schedule the magnitude of water reward was.1,.4, or.8 ml. Persistence during extinction was an increasing function of reward magnitude for the PR animals, but a decreasing function of reward magnitude for the CRF animals. A partial reinforcement extinction effect was observed with the medium and large reward but not with the smallest reward. These data are consistent with the literature dealing with food reward.
Bulletin of the psychonomic society | 1975
Patrick E. Campbell; Mark Cline
Forty-eight rats were trained in a runway under three acquisition schedules: constant immediate reward, partial delay of reward, and constantly delayed reward. Delay occurred in the goalbox for a duration of 20 sec. Acquisition trials were given at one trial a day for 10 days. The constant delay group ran somewhat slower in acquisition than the other two groups. During extinction there were two treatment levels; constant confinement and no confinement. Confinement occurred in the goalbox for 20 sec, and one extinction trial was given each day for 24 days. The animals which were confined during extinction ran significantly slower than the nonconfined animals. The extinction effects of the acquisition schedules included a modest partial delay extinction effect but no evidence that constant delay caused an increase in persistence.
Bulletin of the psychonomic society | 1981
Brian M. Kruger; Patrick E. Campbell; Mark S. Crabtree
Four rats were given free choice of sides in a shuttlebox. Each rat was enclosed in each side for 30 min prior to each daily 1-h choice session. Shocks of 2 sec duration were administered on both sides at irregular intervals. Shocks were preceded by a tone signal on one side. All rats showed a reliable preference for the signaled shock side in the first phase. Reversal of the visual stimuli differentiating the signal side from the nonsignal side eliminated the preference for all rats. Reinstatement of the original relations resulted in the reestablishment of the preference for two rats. In the next phase, changing to scrambled shock from the previously used unscrambled shock eliminated the preference for these two rats. The preference was not reestablished when unscrambled shock was again used in the final phase. The results are interpreted as supporting preparatory response and conditioned aversiveness interpretations of some of the phenomena in signaled shock studies.
Bulletin of the psychonomic society | 1980
Brian M. Kruger; Paul M. Lavin; Patrick E. Campbell; Harry N. Davis
Self-punitive behavior was demonstrated in prepunishment speeds during extinction following shock-escape training in a straight runway under no-dose, low-dose (1-mg/kg), and high-dose (10-mg/kg) methylphenidate conditions. Increased dosage enhanced punished running (shock in the last half of the runway) and nonpunished running. Self-punitive behavior, defined as the difference between punishment conditions with faster running or increased persistence under punishment conditions, was not significantly affected by the drug variable. The results were interpreted as compatible with conditioned fear interpretations of self-punitive behavior but nonsupportive of cognitive interpretations.
Bulletin of the psychonomic society | 1980
Brian M. Kruger; Michael J. Wietzel; Patrick E. Campbell
Self-punitive behavior was demonstrated in prepunishment speeds during extinction following shock escape training in a straight runway under buzzer-CS and under no-CS conditions. The buzzer enhanced punished running (shock in the last half of the runway) and nonpunished running. The buzzer did not significantly enhance self-punitive behavior, defined as the difference between punishment conditions with faster running for punished rats than for nonpunished rats. Introduction of a novel buzzer at the beginning of extinction enhanced both punished and nonpunished running relative to no-CS conditions but had no effect on selfpunitive behavior. Discontinuance of the buzzer CS at the end of acquisition eliminated selfpunitive behavior during extinction. The results were interpreted as supporting explanations of self-punitive behavior in terms of fear conditioning and conflict resolution.
Bulletin of the psychonomic society | 1980
Patrick E. Campbell; Wendy B. Campbell; Brian M. Kruger; Patricia Roberts
In each of two experiments, rats were trained in a runway with either an alternating or an irregular schedule of partial reward. One-third of the animals within each schedule were trained with a small, medium, or large reward. In Experiment 1, the irregular schedule was constructed in such a way that the probability of an R trial following an N trial was.75 and the probability of an N trial following an R trial was also.75. Of course, in an alternating schedule the probabilities are 1.0. The rats learned to run rapidly following N trials and slowly following R trials when trained under either schedule. This effect, however, was greater in the alternating condition and was facilitated by larger rewards. In Experiment 2, the irregular schedule was constructed so that the occurrence of R or N trials was not a reliable predictor of the following trial. Under these conditions, differential running speeds did not develop in the irregular schedule, but the rats in the alternating schedule performed as in Experiment 1. The data are discussed within the context of probability learning and are related to methodological issues of importance to sequential trial experiments.
Bulletin of the psychonomic society | 1978
Patrick E. Campbell; Brian M. Kruger; Catharine Barclay
Two methods of food deprivation used to control hunger motivation were compared on their ability to control body weight and to control response variability in an appetitive conditioning situation. Percent-weight rats received individually adjusted daily rations to maintain them at 85% of free-feeding body weight. Fixed-food rats each received the average daily ration given the percent-weight subjects. All were given 34 days of training on an FR 10 schedule followed by 15 days on a VI 30-sec schedule. There were no significant differences in mean body weights or mean barpressing rates. Variability in body weight was significantly greater for fixed-food rats, but there was no significant difference in barpress variability. Since experimental error was not reduced by the more costly percent-weight method, it is suggested that the fixed-food method be used for experimental conditions similar to those used in the present study.
Bulletin of the psychonomic society | 1977
Patrick E. Campbell; Charles T. Cleveland
Rats were given continuous reinforcement in both alleys of a two-alley runway apparatus. For three groups intermittent punishment occurred in one of the alleys (P alley) but not in the other alley (C alley). A fourth group did not receive punishment but was given control shocks following each daily session. The three punished groups had punished trials followed by rewarded trials in the P alley, the C alley, or in both alleys. The persistence of the animals was measured in both alleys with reward and punishment present on every trial. All of the punished animals ran more slowly in the P alley during acquisition and continued this behavior during the continuous punishment testing phase. All of the punished animals ran more rapidly during the test phase than the animals given only control shocks during testing.