Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Patrick J. O'Donnell is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Patrick J. O'Donnell.


Biological Psychology | 2009

Early emotion word processing: Evidence from event-related potentials

Graham G. Scott; Patrick J. O'Donnell; Hartmut Leuthold; Sara C. Sereno

Behavioral and electrophysiological responses were monitored to 80 controlled sets of emotionally positive, negative, and neutral words presented randomly in a lexical decision paradigm. Half of the words were low frequency and half were high frequency. Behavioral results showed significant effects of frequency and emotion as well as an interaction. Prior research has demonstrated sensitivity to lexical processing in the N1 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). In this study, the N1 (135-180 ms) showed a significant emotion by frequency interaction. The P1 window (80-120 ms) preceding the N1 as well as post-N1 time windows, including the Early Posterior Negativity (200-300 ms) and P300 (300-450 ms), were examined. The ERP data suggest an early identification of the emotional tone of words leading to differential processing. Specifically, high frequency negative words seem to attract additional cognitive resources. The overall pattern of results is consistent with a time line of word recognition in which semantic analysis, including the evaluation of emotional quality, occurs at an early, lexical stage of processing.


The Lancet | 1996

Prevention of hyperacute rejection by removal of antibodies to HLA immediately before renal transplantation

Robert M. Higgins; Deborah J. Bevan; B S Carey; C K Lea; M. Fallon; R Bühler; Robert Vaughan; Patrick J. O'Donnell; Susan A. Snowden; Michael Bewick; Bruce M. Hendry

BACKGROUND Many patients with circulating antibodies to human leucocyte antigens (anti-HLA) are highly sensitised against renal transplantation and are liable to immediate graft loss through hyperacute rejection. Our aim was to find out whether removal of anti-HLA immediately before renal transplantation prevented hyperacute graft rejection. METHODS 13 highly sensitised patients underwent cadaveric renal transplants immediately after immunoadsorption (IA) treatment to remove anti-HLA. Before IA, 12 patients had a positive crossmatch against donor cells either by cytotoxic or flow-cytometric assay; results for one patient were equivocal. FINDINGS Renal biopsy samples were obtained 20 min after removal of the vascular clamps in nine patients. There was no evidence of hyperacute rejection in six of the nine patients; the other three patients showed glomerular thrombosis but no other evidence of hyperacute rejection. Two of these three grafts were functioning at 31 months of follow-up. Six episodes of acute rejection occurred in five patients during the first month after transplantation and overall there were 13 rejection episodes in nine patients. At latest follow-up (median 26 months, range 9-42), 12 of 13 patients were alive and seven of 13 grafts were surviving with a median plasma creatinine concentration of 185 mumol/L (range 106-296) in the functioning grafts. No graft was lost as a result of classic hyperacute rejection. INTERPRETATION Immediate pretransplant IA can prevent hyperacute rejection and provide an opportunity for successful transplantation in highly sensitised patients.


Psychopharmacology | 1999

Level of use of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA or Ecstasy) in humans correlates with EEG power and coherence

Richard I. Dafters; Frances Duffy; Patrick J. O'Donnell; Caroline Bouquet

Rationale: Despite animal studies implicating 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA or Ecstasy) in serotonergic neurotoxicity, there is little direct evidence of changes in neural function in humans who use MDMA as a recreational drug. Objective: The present study investigated whether there is a correlation between quantitative EEG variables (spectral power and coherence) and cognitive/mood variables, and level of prior use of MDMA. Methods: Twenty-three recreational MDMA users were studied. Resting EEG was recorded with eyes closed, using a 128-electrode geodesic net system, from which spectral power, peak frequency and coherence levels were calculated. Tests of intelligence (NART), immediate and delayed memory, frontal function (card sort task), and mood (BDI and PANAS scales) were also administered. Pearson correlation analyses were used to examine the relationship between these measures and the subject’s consumption of MDMA during the previous 12-month period. Partial correlation was used to control for the use of other recreational drugs. Results: MDMA use was positively correlated with absolute power in the alpha (8–12 Hz) and beta (12–20 Hz) frequency bands, but not with the delta (1–3 Hz) or theta (4–7 Hz) bands. MDMA use was negatively correlated with EEG coherence, a measure of synchrony between paired cortical locations, in posterior brain sites thought to overly the main visual association pathways of the occipito-parietal region. MDMA use did not correlate significantly with any of the mood/cognitive measures except the card sort task, with which it was weakly negatively correlated. Conclusions: Alpha power has been shown to be inversely related to mental function and has been used as an indirect measure of brain activation in both normal and abnormal states. Reduced coherence levels have been associated with dysfunctional connectivity in the brain in disorders such as dementia, white-matter disease and normal aging. Our results may indicate altered brain function correlated with prior MDMA use, and show that electroencephalography may be a cheap and effective tool for examining neurotoxic effects of MDMA and other drugs.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 2006

Eye movements and lexical ambiguity resolution: Investigating the subordinate bias effect

Sara C. Sereno; Patrick J. O'Donnell; Keith Rayner

Recent debates on lexical ambiguity resolution have centered on the subordinate-bias effect, in which reading time is longer on a biased ambiguous word in a subordinate-biasing context than on a control word. The nature of the control word--namely, whether it matched the frequency of the ambiguous words overall word form or its contextually instantiated word meaning (a higher or lower frequency word, respectively)--was examined. In addition, contexts that were singularly supportive of the ambiguous words subordinate meaning were used. Eye movements were recorded as participants read contextually biasing passages that contained an ambiguous word target or a word-form or word-meaning control. A comparison of fixation times on the 2 control words revealed a significant effect of word frequency. Fixation times on the ambiguous word generally fell between those on the 2 controls and were significantly different than both. Results are discussed in relation to the reordered access model, in which both meaning frequency and prior context affect access procedures.


Psychological Science | 2009

Parafoveal Magnification Visual Acuity Does Not Modulate the Perceptual Span in Reading

Sebastien R Miellet; Patrick J. O'Donnell; Sara C. Sereno

Models of eye guidance in reading rely on the concept of the perceptual span—the amount of information perceived during a single eye fixation, which is considered to be a consequence of visual and attentional constraints. To directly investigate attentional mechanisms underlying the perceptual span, we implemented a new reading paradigm—parafoveal magnification (PM)—that compensates for how visual acuity drops off as a function of retinal eccentricity. On each fixation and in real time, parafoveal text is magnified to equalize its perceptual impact with that of concurrent foveal text. Experiment 1 demonstrated that PM does not increase the amount of text that is processed, supporting an attentional-based account of eye movements in reading. Experiment 2 explored a contentious issue that differentiates competing models of eye movement control and showed that, even when parafoveal information is enlarged, visual attention in reading is allocated in a serial fashion from word to word.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2012

Emotion Words Affect Eye Fixations during Reading.

Graham G. Scott; Patrick J. O'Donnell; Sara C. Sereno

Emotion words are generally characterized as possessing high arousal and extreme valence and have typically been investigated in paradigms in which they are presented and measured as single words. This study examined whether a words emotional qualities influenced the time spent viewing that word in the context of normal reading. Eye movements were monitored as participants read sentences containing an emotionally positive (e.g., lucky), negative (e.g., angry), or neutral (e.g., plain) word. Target word frequency (high or low) was additionally varied to help determine the temporal locus of emotion effects, with interactive results suggesting an early lexical locus of emotion processing. In general, measures of target fixation time demonstrated significant effects of emotion and frequency as well as an interaction. The interaction arose from differential effects with negative words that were dependent on word frequency. Fixation times on emotion words (positive or negative) were consistently faster than those on neutral words with one exception-high-frequency negative words were read no faster than their neutral counterparts. These effects emerged in the earliest eye movement measures, namely, first and single fixation duration, suggesting that emotionality, as defined by arousal and valence, modulates lexical processing. Possible mechanisms involved in processing emotion words are discussed, including automatic vigilance and desensitization, both of which imply a key role for word frequency. Finally, it is important that early lexical effects of emotion processing can be established within the ecologically valid context of fluent reading.


BMJ | 1998

Late onset interstitial nephritis associated with mesalazine treatment

Joyce Popoola; Andrew F. Muller; Lucy Pollock; Patrick J. O'Donnell; Paul Carmichael; Paul E. Stevens

Patients taking mesalazine should have renal function monitored regularly to avoid nephrotoxicity Mesalazine is widely prescribed for the treatment of inflammatory bowel disease. It is a single molecule of 5-aminosalicylic acid (5-ASA), and is structurally similar to phenacetin and aspirin. Occasionally, treatment with mesalazine may lead to a severe indolent interstitial nephritis causing appreciable morbidity. Unless detected and treated early this may progress to end stage renal failure despite withdrawal of the drug.1 It is obvious from the increasing number of reports of nephritis and renal failure occurring after treatment with mesalazine that the premise that “there is no need for routine monitoring of renal function”2 needs to be reviewed; the need for a review has been suggested by a number of recent reports. 1 3–9 We report two cases of late onset interstitial nephritis induced by mesalazine (Asacol); the first presented after at least 5 years of continuous treatment with the drug and the second after 1 year. ### Case 1 A 38 year old laboratory technician began taking mesalazine for ulcerative colitis. After 2 years of continuous treatment he remained well with normal renal function (serum creatinine concentration 76 μmol/l; normal range 71-133 μmol/l) and negative results on urinalysis. He had an exacerbation of his colitis during the third and fourth years of treatment. On each occasion he responded to a combination of oral prednisolone treatment and an increase in the dose of mesalazine to 1.2 g twice a day. Each time, steroid treatment lasted for 3 months and began with 40 mg a day of prednisolone which was rapidly tapered down to a maintenance dose of 10 mg a day. Repeat serum creatinine concentration measured after 3 years of mesalazine treatment was 79 μmol/l. Thereafter the dose of mesalazine fluctuated between 800 …


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 2010

The frequency-predictability interaction in reading: It depends where you're coming from

Christopher J. Hand; Sebastien R Miellet; Patrick J. O'Donnell; Sara C. Sereno

A words frequency of occurrence and its predictability from a prior context are key factors determining how long the eyes remain on that word in normal reading. Past reaction-time and eye movement research can be distinguished by whether these variables, when combined, produce interactive or additive results, respectively. Our study addressed possible methodological limitations of prior experiments. Initial results showed additive effects of frequency and predictability. However, we additionally examined launch site (the distance from the pretarget fixation to the target) to index the extent of parafoveal target processing. Analyses revealed both additive and interactive effects on target fixations, with the nature of the interaction depending on the quality of the parafoveal preview. Target landing position and pretarget fixation time were also considered. Results were interpreted in terms of models of language processing and eye movement control. Our findings with respect to parafoveal preview and fixation time constraints aim to help parameterize eye movement behavior.


technical symposium on computer science education | 2010

Manipulating mindset to positively influence introductory programming performance

Quintin I. Cutts; Emily Cutts; Stephen W. Draper; Patrick J. O'Donnell; Peter Saffrey

Introductory programming classes are renowned for their high dropout rates. The authors propose that this is because students learn to adopt a fixed mindset towards programming. This paper reports on a study carried out with an introductory programming class, based on Dwecks mindset research. Combinations of three interventions were carried out: tutors taught mindset to students; growth mindset feedback messages were given to students on their work; and, when stuck, students were encouraged to use a crib sheet with pathways to solve problems. The study found that the mixture of teaching mindset and giving mindset messages on returned work resulted in a significant change in mindset and a corresponding significant change in test scores - improvements in test scores were found in a class test given immediately after the six-week intervention and at the end-of-year exam. The authors discuss the results and the strengths and weaknesses of the study.


Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology | 2009

Size matters: Bigger is faster

Sara C. Sereno; Patrick J. O'Donnell; Margaret E. Sereno

A largely unexplored aspect of lexical access in visual word recognition is “semantic size”—namely, the real-world size of an object to which a word refers. A total of 42 participants performed a lexical decision task on concrete nouns denoting either big or small objects (e.g., bookcase or teaspoon). Items were matched pairwise on relevant lexical dimensions. Participants’ reaction times were reliably faster to semantically “big” versus “small” words. The results are discussed in terms of possible mechanisms, including more active representations for “big” words, due to the ecological importance attributed to large objects in the environment and the relative speed of neural responses to large objects.

Collaboration


Dive into the Patrick J. O'Donnell's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Caroline Sabin

University College London

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Catherine Horsfield

Guy's and St Thomas' NHS Foundation Trust

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Frank Post

University of Cambridge

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

John B. Davies

University of Strathclyde

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

John W Booth

University College London

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge