Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Philip A. Vernon is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Philip A. Vernon.


Intelligence | 1983

Speed of Information Processing and General Intelligence.

Philip A. Vernon

Abstract This study investigates the relationship between a number of measures of speed of cognitive information-processing and intelligence test scores. One hundred university students were given five tests of speed-of-processing, measuring their speed of encoding, short-term memory scanning, long-term memory retrieval, efficiency of short-term memory storage and processing, and simple and choice reaction time or decision-making speed. They were also given the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale and the Raven Advanced Progression Matrices. A number of multiple regression analyses show that the cognitive processing measures are significantly related to IQ scores. Other analyses indicate that this relationship cannot be attributed to the common content shared by the reaction time and the intelligence tests, nor to the fact that parts of the WAIS are timed. It is concluded that the reaction time tests measure basic cognitive operations which are involved in many forms of intellectual behavior, and that individual differences in intelligence can be attributed, to a moderate extent, to variance in the speed or efficiency with which individuals can execute these operations.


Personality and Individual Differences | 1984

Individual and group differences in intelligence and speed of information processing

Philip A. Vernon; Arthur R. Jensen

Summary-A battery of eight different reaction time (RT) tests, measuring the speed with which individuals perform various elementary cognitive processes, and a group test of scholastic aptitude (the Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery, ASVAB) were given to 50 black and 56 white male vocational college students. The regression of the general factor scores of the ASVAB on the RT measures yielded a shrunken multiple correlation of 0.465. Although discriminant analyses, when applied separately to the ASVAB subtests and to the RT variables, showed highly comparable overall discrimination (over 70% correct classification) between the black and white groups, factor scores derived from the general factor (labeled ‘speed of information processing’) of the RT battery show only about one-third as large a mean black-white difference as the mean group difference on the general factor scores derived from the ASVAB. Comparisons were also made between the 106 vocational college students and 100 university students of higher average academic aptitude who had previously been tested on the same RT batter; IVernon, 1983a). These ErouDs showed marked differences on the RT variables. the largest differences occurring on the tests that required more complex cognitive processing. The more complex RT tests also correlate most highly with the psychometric measures of ability within each group. The results are consistent with the hypothesis that individual differences and the mean differences between groups in psychometric abilities and scholastic achievement are related to differences in the speed of information processing as measured in elementary cognitive tasks.


Personality and Individual Differences | 2002

Genetic and environmental influences on the covariance of facets defining the domains of the five-factor model of personality

Kerry L. Jang; W. John Livesley; Alois Angleitner; Rainer Riemann; Philip A. Vernon

Multivariate genetic analyses were applied to the six facets defining each of the five personality domains (Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness) assessed by Costa and McCraes Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R). The analyses are designed to partition the observed covariance of facets defining each domain into their genetic and environmental bases to determine the basis for their coherence as a domain. The analyses were applied separately to a sample of 253 identical and 207 fraternal twin pairs from Canada and 526 identical and 269 fraternal pairs from Germany. Results showed that each of the NEO-PI-R domains is composed of multiple genetic and environmental factors common to the facets supporting the observed coherence of the NEO-PI-R facet sets. Differences between the German and Canadian sample appeared limited to the magnitude of the genetic and environmental effects on each facet, but not the number or type of genetic and environmental influences.


Hormones and Behavior | 2006

Finger-length ratios show evidence of prenatal hormone-transfer between opposite-sex twins.

Sari M. van Anders; Philip A. Vernon; Christopher J. Wilbur

Finger-length ratio (second to fourth finger; 2D:4D) has been associated with various measures thought to be related to prenatal androgens. In addition, hormone-transfer theory posits that hormones can transfer between twins. We examined 2D:4D in same-sex (SS) and opposite-sex (OS) dizygotic twins to test both propositions. Results show that 2D:4D is masculinized in OS females compared to SS females. This provides strong evidence that 2D:4D is laid down prenatally, and that hormones (likely androgens) can transfer from male to female fetuses. Implications for developmental timeframes for both hormone-transfer and 2D:4D are discussed.


Behavior Genetics | 1998

The heritability of testosterone: A study of Dutch adolescent twins and their parents

Julie Aitken Harris; Philip A. Vernon; Dorret I. Boomsma

The heritability of total plasma testosterone levels, determined from blood samples, was examined in 160 adolescent twin pairs and their parents. Subjects were tested as part of a larger study of cardiovascular risk factors, conducted in Amsterdam. Each subject provided a sample of blood which was assayed to measure testosterone concentrations. Correlations of testosterone in monozygotic twins were higher than in dizygotic twins. No resemblance was found between testosterone values in fathers and those in their children and a moderate correlation was seen between mothers and their daughters. The lack of resemblance between family members of opposite sex suggests that different genetic factors influence plasma testosterone concentrations in men and women. In adolescent men, approximately 60% of the variance in testosterone levels is heritable. The lack of father–son resemblance suggests that different genetic factors may be expressed in adolescence and adulthood. In women, 40% of the variance in testosterone levels is heritable, both in adolescence and in adulthood.


Personality and Individual Differences | 2000

Relationships between factors of intelligence and brain volume

John C. Wickett; Philip A. Vernon; Donald H. Lee

The recent explosion of studies aimed at determining the biological basis of intelligence has revealed that cognitive ability has a strong biological substrate. This study expands on this literature by examining the relation between cognitive ability and MRI-measured brain volume and head size in a sample of adult male siblings recruited from the London, Ontario community in Canada. It was found that brain volume correlated with IQ at 0.35 (P<0.01), thus replicating the results of past studies. Corrections for restriction of range and attenuation in both this and past studies suggest that the population value of the brain volume-IQ correlation is closer to 0.50. Head size variables, with one exception, also showed the expected positive correlations with IQ. The results of a vector analysis on factor scores indicated that the more highly g-loaded a test was the more highly it correlated with brain volume (r=0.59, P<0.01). The sum of the data suggested that although brain volume (and to a lesser extent, head size) is predictive of g, fluid ability, and memory, it does not predict crystallized ability. Unexpectedly, the higher the spatial imaging loading of a test, the less its correlation with brain volume (vector correlation=−0.84, P<0.001).


Twin Research and Human Genetics | 2009

A general factor of personality from multitrait-multimethod data and cross-national twins

J. Philippe Rushton; Trudy Ann Bons; Juko Ando; Yoon-Mi Hur; Paul Irwing; Philip A. Vernon; K. V. Petrides; Claudio Barbaranelli

In three studies, a General Factor of Personality (GFP) was found to occupy the apex of the hierarchical structure. In Study 1, a GFP emerged independent of method variance and accounted for 54% of the reliable variance in a multitrait-multimethod assessment of 391 Italian high school students that used self-, teacher-, and parent-ratings on the Big Five Questionnaire - Children. In Study 2, a GFP was found in the seven dimensions of Cloningers Temperament and Character Inventory as well as the Big Five of the NEO PI-R, with the GFPtci correlating r = .72 with the GFPneo. These results indicate that the GFP is practically the same in both test batteries, and its existence does not depend on being extracted using the Big Five model. The GFP accounted for 22% of the total variance in these trait measures, which were assessed in 651 pairs of 14- to 30-year-old Japanese twins. In Study 3, a GFP accounted for 32% of the total variance in nine scales derived from the NEO PI-R, the Humor Styles Questionnaire, and the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire assessed in 386 pairs of 18- to 74-year-old Canadian and U.S. twins. The GFP was found to be 50% heritable with high scores indicating openness, conscientiousness, sociability, agreeableness, emotional stability, good humor and emotional intelligence. The possible evolutionary origins of the GFP are discussed.


Intelligence | 1992

Intelligence, Reaction Times, and Peripheral Nerve Conduction Velocity.

Philip A. Vernon; Monica Mori

Abstract Two studies (with sample sizes of 85 and 88) are reported that investigated relationships among measures of intelligence, speed of information processing, and peripheral nerve conduction velocity (NCV). In both studies, NCV was significantly correlated with IQ scores (rs = .42 and .48) and with reaction times (RTs; rs = −.28 and −.18): Thus, faster NCV was associated with higher IQ scores and faster speed of processing. In both studies, NCV and RTs contributed significantly, in combination, to the prediction of fullscale IQ (shrunken multiple Rs = .53 and .57), but the expected pattern of causal relationships between the variables was not borne out. The results are interpreted in terms of a “neural efficiency” model of intelligence, which has recieved support from other studies of physiological correlates of human intelligence.


Twin Research and Human Genetics | 2009

Evidence for a heritable general factor of personality in two studies.

Livia Veselka; Julie Aitken Schermer; K. V. Petrides; Philip A. Vernon

Two studies were conducted to see whether a general factor of personality (GFP) could be extracted from different measures of personality. Using samples of twins in both studies also allowed an assessment of the extent to which genetic and/ or environmental factors contributed to individual differences in the GFPs that were found. In Study 1, principal components analysis of the Big Five personality traits in combination with four scales of mental toughness yielded a strong GFP and behavior genetic model-fitting showed that individual differences in this GFP were fully accounted for by genetic and nonshared environmental factors. In Study 2, a GFP was extracted from the Big Five traits in combination with 15 facets of emotional intelligence. Individual differences in this GFP were also fully accounted for by genetic and nonshared environmental factors. These studies add to the growing body of research demonstrating the existence of a GFP and replicate one previous report of its heritability.


Personality and Individual Differences | 1994

In vivo brain size, head perimeter, and intelligence in a sample of healthy adult females

John C. Wickett; Philip A. Vernon; Donald H. Lee

Abstract The issue of the connection between brain size and intelligence has long been of interest to psychology. A review of past research using external head size measures as estimates of internal brain size indicates that head size-IQ correlations are typically in the order of r = 0.10 to 0.30(mean r = 0.194). Today, a more direct and accurate measure of brain size is afforded by the advent of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). In the present study the relationship between brain size (measured via MRI), head size, and intelligence was examined in a sample of 40 healthy, right-handed females (ages 20 to 30 years). Whereas head size correlated r = 0.109 (NS) with full scale IQ, brain size and IQ correlated r = 0.395 (P

Collaboration


Dive into the Philip A. Vernon's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Julie Aitken Schermer

University of Western Ontario

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kerry L. Jang

University of British Columbia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Livia Veselka

University of Western Ontario

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Julie Aitken Harris

University of Western Ontario

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Andrew M. Johnson

University of Western Ontario

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

W. John Livesley

University of British Columbia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

K. V. Petrides

University College London

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Holly M. Baughman

University of Western Ontario

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Rod A. Martin

University of Western Ontario

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

John C. Wickett

University of Western Ontario

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge