Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Philip M. Grove is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Philip M. Grove.


Journal of Vision | 2007

Staying focused: A functional account of perceptual suppression during binocular rivalry

Derek H. Arnold; Philip M. Grove; Thomas S. A. Wallis

Presenting different images to either eye can induce perceptual switching, with alternating disappearances of each image--a phenomenon called binocular rivalry. We believe that disappearances during binocular rivalry can be driven by a process that facilitates visibility near the point of fixation. As the point of fixation is tied neither to a particular stimulus nor to a specific eye, indifference to both would be an essential characteristic for the process we envisage. Many factors that influence disappearances during binocular rivalry scale with distance in depth from fixation. Of these, here we use blur. We break the links between this cue and both eye of origin and stimulus type. We find that perceptual dominance can track a better focused image as it is swapped between the eyes and that perceptual switches can be driven by alternating the focus of images fixed in each eye. This implies that, as a determinant of suppression selectivity, blur is functionally independent from both eye of origin and stimulus type. Our data and theoretical account suggest that binocular rivalry is not an irrelevant laboratory curiosity but, rather, that it is a product of a functional adaptation that promotes visibility in cluttered environments.


Vision Research | 2002

Content and context of monocular regions determine perceived depth in random dot, unpaired background and phantom stereograms

Philip M. Grove; Barbara Gillam; Hiroshi Ono

Perceived depth was measured for three-types of stereograms with the colour/texture of half-occluded (monocular) regions either similar to or dissimilar to that of binocular regions or background. In a two-panel random dot stereogram the monocular region was filled with texture either similar or different to the far panel or left blank. In unpaired background stereograms the monocular region either matched the background or was different in colour or texture and in phantom stereograms the monocular region matched the partially occluded object or was a different colour or texture. In all three cases depth was considerably impaired when the monocular texture did not match either the background or the more distant surface. The content and context of monocular regions as well as their position are important in determining their role as occlusion cues and thus in three-dimensional layout. We compare coincidence and accidental view accounts of these effects.


Annals of Surgery | 2016

The Conflicting Evidence of Three-dimensional Displays in Laparoscopy: A Review of Systems Old and New.

Shinichiro Sakata; Marcus Watson; Philip M. Grove; Andrew R. L. Stevenson

Objective:To describe studies evaluating 3 generations of three-dimensional (3D) displays over the course of 20 years. Summary Background Data:Most previous studies have analyzed performance differences during 3D and two-dimensional (2D) laparoscopy without using appropriate controls that equated conditions in all respects except for 3D or 2D viewing. Methods:Databases search consisted of MEDLINE and PubMed. The reference lists for all relevant articles were also reviewed for additional articles. The search strategy employed the use of keywords “3D,” “Laparoscopic,” “Laparoscopy,” “Performance,” “Education,” “Learning,” and “Surgery” in appropriate combinations. Results:Our current understanding of the performance metrics between 3D and 2D laparoscopy is mostly from the research with flawed study designs. This review has been written in a qualitative style to explain in detail how prior research has underestimated the potential benefit of 3D displays and the improvements that must be made in future experiments comparing 3D and 2D displays to better determine any advantage of using one display or the other. Conclusions:Individual laparoscopic performance in 3D may be affected by a multitude of factors. It is crucial for studies to measure participant stereoscopic ability, control for system crosstalk, and use validated measures of performance.


Perception | 1999

Ecologically Invalid Monocular Texture Leads to Longer Perceptual Latencies in Random-Dot Stereograms

Philip M. Grove; Hiroshi Ono

Two experiments were conducted to explore Gillam and Borstings (1988, Perception 17 603–608) report that uncorrelated monocular texture facilitates stereopsis by shortening the latency to see depth in random-dot stereograms. Experiment 1 used stereograms similar, in pattern but not disparity, to Gillam and Borstings with monocular texture present or absent. A third condition, where monocular texture was dissimilar to the binocular panels and background, was also used. We were unable to generalize the findings of Gillam and Borsting for a depth step of 6 min of arc to a larger depth step of 24 min of arc. That is, we observed no significant difference in latencies between the conditions with monocular texture absent and present at a disparity of 24 min of arc. We found latencies to be significantly longer in the monocular-texture-different condition than the monocular-texture-absent condition, however. We account for this, ad hoc, by arguing that the monocular-texture-different stereogram depicts a rare or ‘accidental’ visual scenario. This account was supported by the results of experiment 2 which showed that stereograms depicting accidental views yielded longer latencies than those depicting generic views. We conclude that the ecological validity of monocular texture must also be considered when assessing the effects of monocular texture on stereopsis.


Perception | 2009

Auditory induced bounce perception persists as the probability of a motion reversal is reduced.

Philip M. Grove; Kenzo Sakurai

When two identical targets move toward one another from opposite sides of a display and continue past one another along collinear trajectories, they can be perceived to either stream past or bounce off of one another. Streaming is the dominant perception in motion displays free of additional transients, while bouncing predominates when a transient (eg auditory or visual) is presented at the point of coincidence. We investigated whether the auditory induced bias towards bouncing would persist as the probability of a motion reversal was reduced by introducing a spatial offset either vertically in a 2-D display or in depth in a 3-D display. Offset conditions were combined with two auditory conditions (tone or no-tone at the point of coincidence) in the presence or absence of a central occluder. In conditions with no sound, streaming was reported on a clear majority of trials, regardless of spatial offset. When a transient tone was presented, reported motion reversals dominated and persisted for increasing verbal offsets up to 17.9 min of arc and for 3-D trajectory offsets up to 25.6 min of arc. The bounce-promoting effect of an auditory tone at the point of coincidence in stream/bounce displays persists in spite of rendering the visual motion sequence unambiguous and more consistent with streaming.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: General | 2003

Leonardo's constraint: two opaque objects cannot be seen in the same direction.

Hiroshi Ono; Linda Lillakas; Philip M. Grove; Masahiro Suzuki

Given Leonardos constraint that 2 opaque objects cannot be seen in the same direction, how are the regions of objects occluded to 1 eye included in perception? To answer this question, the authors presented 3-dimensional stimuli, similar to the ones that concerned Leonardo, and measured the visual directions of their monocular and binocular regions. When the distance between near and far objects was large, the nonfixated object was seen as double and blurry. Leonardos constraint was met by seeing the near object as double and transparent or the distant object as double and superimposed. When the distance between near and far objects was small, the constraint was met by a perceptual displacement and compression of parts of the nonfixated object.


Vision Research | 2004

Slant or occlusion: global factors resolve stereoscopic ambiguity in sets of horizontal lines

Barbara Gillam; Philip M. Grove

Perceived slant was measured for horizontal lines aligned on one side and of varying lengths whose length disparity was either a constant linear amount for all lines (consistent with uniocular occlusion) or proportional to line length (consistent with global slant). Although the disparity of any line was ambiguous with respect to these two possibilities, slant of individual lines did not occur in the former case, but a subjective contour in depth was reported along the alignment. For proportional disparity of the set, global slant was seen. Adding a constant length to each line on the invalid eye for occlusion resulted in multiple slants. Smooth uniocular variations in alignment shape elicited subjective contours slanting or curving in depth. Global context can disambiguate the depth status of individual disparate lines.


Journal of Vision | 2013

Segmentation by depth does not always facilitate visual search

Nonie Finlayson; Roger W. Remington; James D. Retell; Philip M. Grove

In visual search, target detection times are relatively insensitive to set size when targets and distractors differ on a single feature dimension. Search can be confined to only those elements sharing a single feature, such as color (Egeth, Virzi, & Garbart, 1984). These findings have been taken as evidence that elementary feature dimensions support a parallel segmentation of a scene into discrete sets of items. Here we explored if relative depth (signaled by binocular disparity) could support a similar parallel segmentation by examining the effects of distributing distracting elements across two depth planes. Three important empirical findings emerged. First, when the target was a feature singleton on the target depth plane, but a conjunction search among distractors on the nontarget plane, search efficiency increased compared to a single depth plane. Second, benefits of segmentation in depth were only observed when the target depth plane was known in advance. Third, no benefit of segmentation in depth was observed when both planes required a conjunction search, even with prior knowledge of the target depth plane. Overall, the benefit of distributing the elements of a search set across two depth planes was observed only when the two planes differed both in binocular disparity and in the elementary feature composition of individual elements. We conclude that segmentation of the search array into two depth planes can facilitate visual search, but unlike color or other elementary properties, does not provide an automatic, preattentive segmentation.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 2011

Contour entropy: A new determinant of perceiving ground or a hole

Barbara Gillam; Philip M. Grove

Figure-ground perception is typically described as seeing one surface occluding another. Figure properties, not ground properties, are considered the significant factors. In scenes, however, a near surface will often occlude multiple contours and surfaces, often at different depths, producing alignments that are improbable except under conditions of occlusion. We thus hypothesized that unrelated (high entropy) lines would tend to appear as ground in a figure-ground paradigm more often than similarly aligned ordered (low entropy) lines. We further hypothesized that for lines spanning a closed area, high line entropy should increase the hole-like appearance of that area. These predictions were confirmed in three experiments. The probability that patterned rectangles were seen as ground when alternated with blank rectangles increased with pattern entropy. A single rectangular shape appeared more hole-like when the entropy of the enclosed contours increased. Furthermore, these same contours, with the outline shape removed, gave rise to bounding illusory contours whose strength increased with contour entropy. We conclude that figure-ground and hole perception can be determined by properties of ground in the absence of any figural shape, or surround, factors.


Perception | 2001

The Backward Inclination of a Surface Defined by Empirical Corresponding Points

Philip M. Grove; Hirohiko Kaneko; Hiroshi Ono

Three experiments were conducted to investigate whether a locus of binocular correspondence extends eccentrically from the vertical horopter. In experiment 1, we investigated whether the backward inclination of the vertical horopter was manifested in the angle at which readers prefer to orient the page. All observers preferred a page inclined backwards to any other orientation. This backward inclination was less than predicted from previous psychophysical reports, however. In experiment 2, we investigated the extent of binocular correspondence, defined by minimal apparent interocular horizontal motion, in the central 24 deg of the binocular field. Our data define a planar surface inclined top-away from the observer as a locus from which psychophysical corresponding points are stimulated. In experiment 3, we measured vertical adjustments required to eliminate apparent vertical motion. Together, the pattern of results from experiments 2 and 3 is most consistent with a planar surface, inclined top-away from the observer. This is consistent with Helmholtzs account of the backward inclination of the vertical horopter and expands the locus of zero horizontal disparity from a single line in the median plane to eccentric loci extending at least ±12 deg in the central binocular field.

Collaboration


Dive into the Philip M. Grove's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Barbara Gillam

University of New South Wales

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kenzo Sakurai

Tohoku Gakuin University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Andrew R. L. Stevenson

Royal Brisbane and Women's Hospital

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Hirohiko Kaneko

Tokyo Institute of Technology

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge