Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Qingqing Qu is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Qingqing Qu.


Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | 2012

Sound-sized segments are significant for Mandarin speakers

Qingqing Qu; Markus F. Damian; Nina Kazanina

Do speakers of all languages use segmental speech sounds when they produce words? Existing models of language production generally assume a mental representation of individual segmental units, or phonemes, but the bulk of evidence comes from speakers of European languages in which the orthographic system codes explicitly for speech sounds. By contrast, in languages with nonalphabetical scripts, such as Mandarin Chinese, individual speech sounds are not orthographically represented, raising the possibility that speakers of these languages do not use phonemes as fundamental processing units. We used event-related potentials (ERPs) combined with behavioral measurement to investigate the role of phonemes in Mandarin production. Mandarin native speakers named colored line drawings of objects using color adjective-noun phrases; color and object name either shared the initial phoneme or were phonologically unrelated. Whereas naming latencies were unaffected by phoneme repetition, ERP responses were modulated from 200 ms after picture onset. Our ERP findings thus provide strong support for the claim that phonemic segments constitute fundamental units of phonological encoding even for speakers of languages that do not encode such units orthographically.


Psychological Science | 2011

Phonology Contributes to Writing Evidence From Written Word Production in a Nonalphabetic Script

Qingqing Qu; Markus F. Damian; Qingfang Zhang; Xuebing Zhu

Is the production of written words affected by their phonological properties? Most researchers agree that orthographic codes can be accessed directly from meaning, but the contribution of phonological codes to written word production remains controversial, mainly because studies have focused on languages with alphabetic scripts, and it is difficult to dissociate sound from spelling in such languages. We report results from a picture-word interference task in which Chinese participants wrote the names of pictures while attempting to ignore written distractor words. On some trials, the distractors were phonologically and orthographically related to the picture names; on other trials, the distractors were only phonologically related to the picture names; and on still other trials, the distractors and picture names were unrelated. Priming effects were found for both types of related distractors relative to unrelated distractors. This result constitutes clear evidence that phonological properties constrain orthographic output. Additionally, the results speak to the nature of Chinese orthography, suggesting subsemantic correspondences between sound and spelling.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2013

Is handwriting constrained by phonology? Evidence from Stroop tasks with written responses and Chinese characters

Markus F. Damian; Qingqing Qu

To what extent is handwritten word production based on phonological codes? A few studies conducted in Western languages have recently provided evidence showing that phonology contributes to the retrieval of graphemic properties in written output tasks. Less is known about how orthographic production works in languages with non-alphabetic scripts such as written Chinese. We report a Stroop study in which Chinese participants wrote the color of characters on a digital graphic tablet; characters were either neutral, or homophonic to the target (congruent), or homophonic to an alternative (incongruent). Facilitation was found from congruent homophonic distractors, but only when the homophone shared the same tone with the target. This finding suggests a contribution of phonology to written word production. A second experiment served as a control experiment to exclude the possibility that the effect in Experiment 1 had an exclusively semantic locus. Overall, the findings offer new insight into the relative contribution of phonology to handwriting, particularly in non-Western languages.


Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | 2013

Reply to O’Seaghdha et al.: Primary phonological planning units in Chinese are phonemically specified

Qingqing Qu; Markus F. Damian; Nina Kazanina

Whereas O’Seaghdha et al. (1) generally agree with our argument that phoneme-sized segments are implicated in Chinese language production (2), they raise two critical points. First, they argue that our main event-related potential (ERP) finding (i.e., ERP modulation resulting from initial phoneme repetition) may reflect phonological connectivity rather than functional involvement of phonemes during speech planning. We believe, however, that our ERP effects cannot be accounted for via phonological connectivity within the proximate unit framework that is espoused by these researchers. Recall that in their framework (3), the phonological encoding stage is operationalized exclusively in terms of atonal syllables, which are indivisible units without any internal structure (ref. 3, p. 297). Phonemes are not activated until a later (“penultimate”) stage at which the tone and segmental context of an atonal syllable are specified, which directly precedes articulation of speech (which begins approximately 1,000 ms after stimulus onset in our study). Whereas the temporal location of our ERP effects (200 to 400 ms after picture onset) is highly compatible with current timing estimates of phonological encoding in Western languages (4), it is rather earlier than phonological connectivity effects could be expected. Theoretically, our findings are compatible with a model in which primary phonological planning units in Chinese are phonemically specified syllables (i.e., Chinese lexical representations accessed during phonological encoding are syllabified, phonemically specified representations). In English, primary phonological planning units are phonemes and representations accessed during phonological encoding are not syllabified.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2015

Cascadedness in Chinese written word production

Qingqing Qu; Markus F. Damian

In written word production, is activation transmitted from lexical-semantic selection to orthographic encoding in a serial or cascaded fashion? Very few previous studies have addressed this issue, and the existing evidence comes from languages with alphabetic orthographic systems. We report a study in which Chinese participants were presented with colored line drawings of objects and were instructed to write the name of the color while attempting to ignore the object. Significant priming was found when on a trial, the written response shared an orthographic radical with the written name of the object. This finding constitutes clear evidence that task-irrelevant lexical codes activate their corresponding orthographic representation, and hence suggests that activation flows in a cascaded fashion within the written production system. Additionally, the results speak to how the time interval between processing of target and distractor dimensions affects and modulates the emergence of orthographic facilitation effects.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 2016

Semantic information mediates visual attention during spoken word recognition in Chinese: Evidence from the printed-word version of the visual-world paradigm

Wei Shen; Qingqing Qu; Xingshan Li

In the present study, we investigated whether the activation of semantic information during spoken word recognition can mediate visual attention’s deployment to printed Chinese words. We used a visual-world paradigm with printed words, in which participants listened to a spoken target word embedded in a neutral spoken sentence while looking at a visual display of printed words. We examined whether a semantic competitor effect could be observed in the printed-word version of the visual-world paradigm. In Experiment 1, the relationship between the spoken target words and the printed words was manipulated so that they were semantically related (a semantic competitor), phonologically related (a phonological competitor), or unrelated (distractors). We found that the probability of fixations on semantic competitors was significantly higher than that of fixations on the distractors. In Experiment 2, the orthographic similarity between the spoken target words and their semantic competitors was manipulated to further examine whether the semantic competitor effect was modulated by orthographic similarity. We found significant semantic competitor effects regardless of orthographic similarity. Our study not only reveals that semantic information can affect visual attention, it also provides important new insights into the methodology employed to investigate the semantic processing of spoken words during spoken word recognition using the printed-word version of the visual-world paradigm.


Language, cognition and neuroscience | 2015

Phonology contributes to writing: evidence from a masked priming task

Qingqing Qu; Markus F. Damian; Xingshan Li

ABSTRACT Is written word production affected by phonological properties of target words? We report three experiments using masked priming to investigate this issue. Chinese was chosen as the target script because sound and spelling can be largely dissociated. Participants wrote down names of objects, and latencies were measured on a graphic tablet. Objects were preceded by masked prime words which were either phonologically and orthographically related (PO) to the picture name, phonologically related but orthographically unrelated (P), or unrelated. Priming effects were found for both types of related primes with prime exposure durations of 58 ms (Experiment 1) and 33 ms (Experiment 2), with PO priming larger than P priming. Priming disappeared in Experiment 3 when a manual semantic judgment was required instead of written naming, suggesting that facilitation in the earlier experiments originated at the orthographic output level. These findings strengthen the existing evidence for the involvement of phonology in written word production.


Memory & Cognition | 2018

Visual attention shift to printed words during spoken word recognition in Chinese: The role of phonological information

Wei Shen; Qingqing Qu; Xiuhong Tong

The aim of this study was to investigate the extent to which phonological information mediates the visual attention shift to printed Chinese words in spoken word recognition by using an eye-movement technique with a printed-word paradigm. In this paradigm, participants are visually presented with four printed words on a computer screen, which include a target word, a phonological competitor, and two distractors. Participants are then required to select the target word using a computer mouse, and the eye movements are recorded. In Experiment 1, phonological information was manipulated at the full-phonological overlap; in Experiment 2, phonological information at the partial-phonological overlap was manipulated; and in Experiment 3, the phonological competitors were manipulated to share either fulloverlap or partial-overlap with targets directly. Results of the three experiments showed that the phonological competitor effects were observed at both the full-phonological overlap and partial-phonological overlap conditions. That is, phonological competitors attracted more fixations than distractors, which suggested that phonological information mediates the visual attention shift during spoken word recognition. More importantly, we found that the mediating role of phonological information varies as a function of the phonological similarity between target words and phonological competitors.


Psychonomic Bulletin & Review | 2017

The time course of morphological processing during spoken word recognition in Chinese

Wei Shen; Qingqing Qu; Aiping Ni; Junyi Zhou; Xingshan Li

We investigated the time course of morphological processing during spoken word recognition using the printed-word paradigm. Chinese participants were asked to listen to a spoken disyllabic compound word while simultaneously viewing a printed-word display. Each visual display consisted of three printed words: a semantic associate of the first constituent of the compound word (morphemic competitor), a semantic associate of the whole compound word (whole-word competitor), and an unrelated word (distractor). Participants were directed to detect whether the spoken target word was on the visual display. Results indicated that both the morphemic and whole-word competitors attracted more fixations than the distractor. More importantly, the morphemic competitor began to diverge from the distractor immediately at the acoustic offset of the first constituent, which was earlier than the whole-word competitor. These results suggest that lexical access to the auditory word is incremental and morphological processing (i.e., semantic access to the first constituent) that occurs at an early processing stage before access to the representation of the whole word in Chinese.


Psychonomic Bulletin & Review | 2017

Orthographic effects in spoken word recognition: Evidence from Chinese.

Qingqing Qu; Markus F. Damian

Extensive evidence from alphabetic languages demonstrates a role of orthography in the processing of spoken words. Because alphabetic systems explicitly code speech sounds, such effects are perhaps not surprising. However, it is less clear whether orthographic codes are involuntarily accessed from spoken words in languages with non-alphabetic systems, in which the sound-spelling correspondence is largely arbitrary. We investigated the role of orthography via a semantic relatedness judgment task: native Mandarin speakers judged whether or not spoken word pairs were related in meaning. Word pairs were either semantically related, orthographically related, or unrelated. Results showed that relatedness judgments were made faster for word pairs that were semantically related than for unrelated word pairs. Critically, orthographic overlap on semantically unrelated word pairs induced a significant increase in response latencies. These findings indicate that orthographic information is involuntarily accessed in spoken-word recognition, even in a non-alphabetic language such as Chinese.

Collaboration


Dive into the Qingqing Qu's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Wei Shen

Chinese Academy of Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Xingshan Li

Chinese Academy of Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Qingfang Zhang

Chinese Academy of Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Aiping Ni

Chinese Academy of Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Junyi Zhou

Chinese Academy of Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Xuebing Zhu

Shanghai International Studies University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Xiuhong Tong

University of Hong Kong

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge