Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Rachel Coats is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Rachel Coats.


Experimental Brain Research | 2008

Calibrating grasp size and reach distance: interactions reveal integral organization of reaching-to-grasp movements

Rachel Coats; Geoffrey P. Bingham; Mark Mon-Williams

Feedback is a central feature of neural systems and of crucial importance to human behaviour as shown in goal directed actions such as reaching-to-grasp. One important source of feedback in reach-to-grasp behaviour arises from the haptic information obtained after grasping an object. We manipulated the felt distance and/or size of a visually constant object to explore the role of haptic information in the calibration of reaching and grasping. Crucially, our design explored post-adaptation effects rather than the previously documented role of haptic information in movement organisation. A post-adaptation reach-to-grasp task showed: (1) distorted haptic feedback caused recalibration; (2) reach distance and grasp size could be calibrated separately but, if calibrated simultaneously, then (3) recalibration was greater when distance and size changed in a consistent (e.g. reaching for a larger object at a greater distance) rather than an inconsistent (e.g. a smaller object at a greater distance) fashion. These interactions reveal the integral nature of reach-to-grasp organization, that is, that reaching and grasping are integrated components of a single action system.


Experimental Brain Research | 2010

Learning a coordinated rhythmic movement with task-appropriate coordination feedback

Andrew D. Wilson; Winona Snapp-Childs; Rachel Coats; Geoffrey P. Bingham

A common perception–action learning task is to teach participants to produce a novel coordinated rhythmic movement, e.g. 90° mean relative phase. As a general rule, people cannot produce these novel movements stably without training. This is because they are extremely poor at discriminating the perceptual information required to coordinate and control the movement, which means people require additional (augmented) feedback to learn the novel task. Extant methods (e.g. visual metronomes, Lissajous figures) work, but all involve transforming the perceptual information about the task and thus altering the perception–action task dynamic being studied. We describe and test a new method for providing online augmented coordination feedback using a neutral colour cue. This does not alter the perceptual information or the overall task dynamic, and an experiment confirms that (a) feedback is required for learning a novel coordination and (b) the new feedback method provides the necessary assistance. This task-appropriate augmented feedback therefore allows us to study the process of learning while preserving the perceptual information that constitutes a key part of the task dynamic being studied. This method is inspired by and supports a fully perception–action approach to coordinated rhythmic movement.


Experimental Brain Research | 2011

The reliance on visual feedback control by older adults is highlighted in tasks requiring precise endpoint placement and precision grip

Rachel Coats; John P. Wann

There is an ongoing debate as to whether a greater degree of sensory–motor control is required to maintain skills as humans progress to be septuagenarians. Here, we investigate the dependence of older participants upon vision to execute skilled prehension movements. In a first experiment, participants were required to place a small, round peg in one of three randomly cued holes. A mirror apparatus was used to create conditions where they could always see the target locations, but vision of their hand approaching the target could be removed, and we explored end position accuracy. In a second experiment, we examined the ability of participants to precisely control their grasp action under conditions where they could see the objects but not their hands completing the action. The results showed that in Experiment 1, the older adults undershot the target in their primary submovement and hence had to move further in their secondary movement to achieve their goal. In Experiment 2, the older adults spent longer in the final adjustment phase (a near zero velocity phase at the end of the reach) when vision of the hand was unavailable. These findings suggest that older adults are indeed more reliant on visual feedback than the young in tasks that require precise manual control, and this clarifies conflicting accounts in the prior literature.


PLOS ONE | 2012

Reaching a Better Understanding of the Control of Bimanual Movements in Older Adults

Rachel Coats; John P. Wann

The ability to interact skilfully with the environment is essential for independent living and therefore a critical factor for the aging population. Here we investigate the differences between young and older adults in a bimanual reaching task where the goal is to bring two objects together to the same location with a synchronous placement. Older (mean age 74) and young (mean age 20) adults were asked to pick up two spatially disparate objects, one in each hand, and bring them together to place them in one of three trays laid out in front of them from left to right. The results showed that the older adults were no more detrimentally affected than the young by asymmetric bimanual movements compared to symmetric ones, and both groups completed their movements in the same time. Nevertheless, compared to the young, the older adult group produced reaches characterised by higher peak velocities (although this effect was marginal), shorter hover times, and where the movement distance varied for each hand the scaling of the kinematic profile across the two limbs diverged from that found with younger participants. They then spent longer than the young in the final adjustment phase and during this phase they made more adjustments than the young, and as a result were more synchronous in terms of the final placement of the objects. It seems that the older adults produced reach movements that were designed to reach the vicinity of the tray quite rapidly, after which time they made discreet adjustments to their initial trajectories in order to exercise the precision necessary to place the objects in the tray. These findings are consistent with the idea that older adults have problems using online control (as they wait until they can fixate both objects before making adjustments).


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 2014

Perturbation of perceptual units reveals dominance hierarchy in cross calibration.

Rachel Coats; Jing S. Pan; Geoffrey P. Bingham

Bingham and Pagano (1998) argued that calibration is an intrinsic component of perception-action that yields accurate targeted actions. They described calibration as of a mapping from embodied units of perception to embodied units of action. This mapping theory yields a number of predictions. The authors tested 2 of them. The 1st prediction is that change in the size of perceptual units should yield a corresponding change in the slope of the relation between response distances and actual target distances. In Experiment 1, the authors tested this prediction by manipulating interpupillary distance (IPD) as the unit for binocular perception of distance using vergence angles. In Experiment 2, they manipulated eye height (EH) as the unit for monocular perception of distance using elevation angles. In both cases, the results confirmed the predictions. The 2nd prediction was that perceptual units should interact to cross calibrate one another according to a dominance hierarchy among the units. The theory predicts a more temporally stable unit is used to calibrate a less stable unit but not the reverse. EH units change frequently, but IPD units do not, so IPD should be dominant. Simultaneously available IPD and EH units were perturbed successively (without feedback). As predicted, EH was recalibrated by IPD, but IPD was not recalibrated by EH. The mapping among units theory of calibration was thus supported.


Experimental Brain Research | 2013

Perceptuo-motor learning rate declines by half from 20s to 70/80s

Rachel Coats; Winona Snapp-Childs; Andrew D. Wilson; Geoffrey P. Bingham

This study examined perception–action learning in younger adults in their 20s compared to older adults in their 70s and 80s. The goal was to provide, for the first time, quantitative estimates of perceptuo-motor learning rates for each age group and to reveal how these learning rates change between these age groups. We used a visual coordination task in which participants are asked to learn to produce a novel-coordinated rhythmic movement. The task has been studied extensively in young adults, and the characteristics of the task are well understood. All groups showed improvement, although learning rates for those in their 70s and 80s were half the rate for those in their 20s. We consider the potential causes of these differences in learning rates by examining performance across the different coordination patterns examined as well as recent results that reveal age-related deficits in motion perception.


PLOS ONE | 2016

Moving to Capture Children’s Attention: Developing a Methodology for Measuring Visuomotor Attention

Liam J. B. Hill; Rachel Coats; Faisal Mushtaq; Justin H. G. Williams; Lorna Aucott; Mark Mon-Williams

Attention underpins many activities integral to a child’s development. However, methodological limitations currently make large-scale assessment of children’s attentional skill impractical, costly and lacking in ecological validity. Consequently we developed a measure of ‘Visual Motor Attention’ (VMA)—a construct defined as the ability to sustain and adapt visuomotor behaviour in response to task-relevant visual information. In a series of experiments, we evaluated the capability of our method to measure attentional processes and their contributions in guiding visuomotor behaviour. Experiment 1 established the method’s core features (ability to track stimuli moving on a tablet-computer screen with a hand-held stylus) and demonstrated its sensitivity to principled manipulations in adults’ attentional load. Experiment 2 standardised a format suitable for use with children and showed construct validity by capturing developmental changes in executive attention processes. Experiment 3 tested the hypothesis that children with and without coordination difficulties would show qualitatively different response patterns, finding an interaction between the cognitive and motor factors underpinning responses. Experiment 4 identified associations between VMA performance and existing standardised attention assessments and thereby confirmed convergent validity. These results establish a novel approach to measuring childhood attention that can produce meaningful functional assessments that capture how attention operates in an ecologically valid context (i.e. attentions specific contribution to visuomanual action).


Human Movement Science | 2015

Multisensory integration in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder

Rachel Coats; Laura Britten; Andrea Utley; Sarah Astill

This study examines how multisensory stimuli affect the performance of children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) on a choice reaction time (CRT) task. Ten children with DCD, identified using the Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2, aged 7-10 years (4F, M=8 y 3 m, SD=17 m) and 10 typically developing peers (TDC) (5F, M=8 y 4 m, SD=17 m) reached to unimodal (auditory (AO), visual (VO)) and bimodal (audiovisual (AV)) stimuli at one of three target locations. A multisensory (AV) stimulus reduced RTs for both groups (p<0.001, η(2)=0.36). While the children with DCD had a longer RT in all conditions, the AV stimulus produced RTs in children with DCD (494 ms) that were equivalent to those produced by the TDC to the VO stimulus (493 ms). Movement Time (DCD=486 ms; TDC=434 ms) and Path Length (DCD=25.6 cm; TDC=24.2 cm) were longer in children with DCD compared to TDC as expected (p<0.05). Only the TDC benefited from the AV information for movement control, as deceleration time of the dominant hand was seen to decrease when moving to an AV stimulus (p<0.05). Overall, data shows children with DCD do benefit from a bimodal stimulus to plan their movement, but do not for movement control. Further research is required to understand if this is a result of impaired multisensory integration.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 2014

A geometric and dynamic affordance model of reaches-to-grasp: Men take greater risks than women.

Geoffrey P. Bingham; Winona Snapp-Childs; Aaron Fath; Jing Samantha Pan; Rachel Coats

Mon-Williams and Bingham (2011) developed an affordance model of the spatial structure of reaches-to-grasp. With a single free parameter (P), the model predicted the safety margins (SMs) exhibited in maximum grasp apertures (MGAs), during the approach of a hand to a target object, as a function of an affordance measure of object size and a functional measure of hand size. An affordance analysis revealed that object size is determined by a diagonal through the object, called the maximum object extent. Mon-Williams and Bingham provided no theoretical account for the empirically determined values of P. We now address this question. Snapp-Childs and Bingham (2009) augmented Warrens (1984) geometric affordance scaling model with a dynamical component determined by the stability of the motor performance. Because P was found to vary with the speeds of reaches, we incorporated a measure of the variability of performance into the model to yield predictions of P. We also found that P varied with gender. In respect to the size of safety margins, women were more conservative in taking risks then men. Finally, following Warren (1984), the classic paradigm for testing affordance models is to test the scaling relations with both small and large participants. We tested small- and large-handed men and small- and large-handed women and found that the new parameter free model successfully accounted for the spatial structure of reaches-to-grasp.


PLOS ONE | 2017

Bimanual reach to grasp movements after cervical spinal cord injury

Laura Britten; Rachel Coats; Ronaldo M. Ichiyama; Wajid Raza; Firas Jamil; Sarah Astill

Injury to the cervical spinal cord results in bilateral deficits in arm/hand function reducing functional independence and quality of life. To date little research has been undertaken to investigate control strategies of arm/hand movements following cervical spinal cord injury (cSCI). This study aimed to investigate unimanual and bimanual coordination in patients with acute cSCI using 3D kinematic analysis as they performed naturalistic reach to grasp actions with one hand, or with both hands together (symmetrical task), and compare this to the movement patterns of uninjured younger and older adults. Eighteen adults with a cSCI (mean 61.61 years) with lesions at C4-C8, with an American Spinal Injury Association (ASIA) grade B to D and 16 uninjured younger adults (mean 23.68 years) and sixteen uninjured older adults (mean 70.92 years) were recruited. Participants with a cSCI produced reach-to-grasp actions which took longer, were slower, and had longer deceleration phases than uninjured participants. These differences were exacerbated during bimanual reach-to-grasp tasks. Maximal grasp aperture was no different between groups, but reached earlier by people with cSCI. Participants with a cSCI were less synchronous than younger and older adults but all groups used the deceleration phase for error correction to end the movement in a synchronous fashion. Overall, this study suggests that after cSCI a level of bimanual coordination is retained. While there seems to be a greater reliance on feedback to produce both the reach to grasp, we observed minimal disruption of the more impaired limb on the less impaired limb. This suggests that bimanual movements should be integrated into therapy.

Collaboration


Dive into the Rachel Coats's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge