Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Rachel Keen is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Rachel Keen.


Psychological Science | 2003

Evidence of Motor Planning in Infant Reaching Behavior

Laura J. Claxton; Rachel Keen; Michael E. McCarty

When adults reach for an object, kinematic measures of their approach movement are affected by what they intend to do after grasping it. We examined whether such future intended actions would be reflected in the approach-to-grasp phase of infant reaching. Twenty-one 10-month-old infants were encouraged to either throw a ball into a tub or fit it down a tube. Kinematic measures of the approach phase of the reach toward the ball were obtained using a motion analysis system. Infants, like adults, reached for the ball faster if they were going to subsequently throw it as opposed to using it in the precision action. The perceptual aspects of the ball were the same and cannot account for these kinematic differences. Infants appear to be planning both segments of their actions in advance. Our findings provide evidence for a level of sophistication in infant motor planning not reported before.


Current Directions in Psychological Science | 2003

Representation of Objects and Events: Why Do Infants Look So Smart and Toddlers Look So Dumb?

Rachel Keen

Research has demonstrated that very young infants can discriminate between visual events that are physically impossible versus possible. These findings suggest that infants have knowledge of physical laws concerning solidity and continuity. However, research with 2-year-olds has shown that they cannot solve simple problems involving search for a hidden object, even though these problems require the same knowledge. These apparently inconsistent findings raise questions about the interpretation of both data sets. This discrepancy may be resolved by examining differences in task demands.


Annual Review of Psychology | 2011

The Development of Problem Solving in Young Children: A Critical Cognitive Skill

Rachel Keen

Problem solving is a signature attribute of adult humans, but we need to understand how this develops in children. Tool use is proposed as an ideal way to study problem solving in children less than 3 years of age because overt manual action can reveal how the child plans to achieve a goal. Motor errors are as informative as successful actions. Research is reviewed on intentional actions, beginning with block play and progressing to picking up a spoon in different orientations, and finally retrieving objects with rakes and from inside tubes. Behavioral and kinematic measures of motor action are combined to show different facets of skill acquisition and mastery. We need to design environments that encourage and enhance problem solving from a young age. One goal of this review is to excite interest and spur new research on the beginnings of problem solving and its elaboration during development.


Developmental Science | 2003

How infants use perceptual information to guide action

Rachel Keen; Renee L. Carrico; Monica R. Sylvia; Neil E. Berthier

When infants catch a rolling ball by intercepting its trajectory, the action is prospectively controlled to take account of the objects speed, direction and path. We complicated this task in two ways: by occluding a portion of the balls path with a screen, and by sometimes placing a barrier that blocked the balls path behind the screen. In two experiments we manipulated visual information about the barrier and the balls trajectory to see how this would aid 9-month-olds’ performance. Anticipatory reaching was possible but difficult with a partially occluded trajectory; actually catching the ball was aided by full view of the trajectory although timing of reach onset was not affected. Full sight of the barrier and trajectory through a transparent screen prevented inappropriate reaching, whereas sight of the barrier alone through a ‘window’ in an opaque screen did not. We interpreted these results as evidence for decreased performance as cognitive load increased with the loss of visual information. In contrast to anticipatory reaching behavior, search for the ball after it disappeared behind the screen was facilitated by the opaque window condition, confirming previous studies that found superior search with opaque versus transparent screens.


Child Development | 2012

Age Differences in Online Processing of Video: An Eye Movement Study

Heather L. Kirkorian; Daniel R. Anderson; Rachel Keen

Eye movements were recorded while sixty-two 1-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and adults watched television. Of interest was the extent to which viewers looked at the same place at the same time as their peers because high similarity across viewers suggests systematic viewing driven by comprehension processes. Similarity of gaze location increased with age. This was particularly true immediately following a cut to a new scene, partly because older viewers (but not infants) tended to fixate the center of the screen following a cut. Conversely, infants appear to require several seconds to orient to a new scene. Results are interpreted in the context of developing attention skills. Findings have implications for the extent to which infants comprehend and learn from commercial video.


Infant Behavior & Development | 2009

Self-directed action affects planning in tool-use tasks with toddlers

Laura J. Claxton; Michael E. McCarty; Rachel Keen

Toddlers grasp a tool more effectively when it is self-directed (e.g., spoon) than other-directed (e.g., hammer), possibly because the consequences of self-directed actions are more obvious. When the negative consequences of an inefficient grip were made equally salient, the self-directed versus other-directed differences remained.


Child Development | 2010

Movement Planning Reflects Skill Level and Age Changes in Toddlers

Yu-ping Chen; Rachel Keen; Kerstin Rosander; Claes von Hofsten

Kinematic measures of childrens reaching were found to reflect stable differences in skill level for planning for future actions. Thirty-five toddlers (18-21 months) were engaged in building block towers (precise task) and in placing blocks into an open container (imprecise task). Sixteen children were retested on the same tasks a year later. Longer deceleration as the hand approached the block for pickup was found in the tower task compared with the imprecise task, indicating planning for the second movement. More skillful toddlers who could build high towers had a longer deceleration phase when placing blocks on the tower than toddlers who built low towers. Kinematic differences between the groups remained a year later when all children could build high towers.


Child Development | 2011

Imagining a Way Out of the Gravity Bias: Preschoolers Can Visualize the Solution to a Spatial Problem

Amy S. Joh; Vikram K. Jaswal; Rachel Keen

Can young children visualize the solution to a difficult spatial problem? Forty-eight 3-year-olds were tested in a spatial reasoning paradigm in which they were asked to predict the path of a ball moving through 1 of 3 intertwined tubes. One group of children was asked to visualize the ball rolling down the tube before they made their predictions, a second group was given identical instructions without being asked to use visual imagery, and a third group was given no instructions. Children in the visualization condition performed significantly better than those in the other conditions, suggesting that encouraging young children to use visual imagery may help them to reason through difficult problems.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2012

Development of the Coordination between Posture and Manual Control.

Jeffrey M. Haddad; Laura J. Claxton; Rachel Keen; Neil E. Berthier; Gary E. Riccio; Joseph Hamill; Richard E.A. van Emmerik

Studies have suggested that proper postural control is essential for the development of reaching. However, little research has examined the development of the coordination between posture and manual control throughout childhood. We investigated the coordination between posture and manual control in children (7- and 10-year-olds) and adults during a precision fitting task as task constraints became more difficult. Participants fit a block through an opening as arm kinematics, trunk kinematics, and center of pressure data were collected. During the fitting task, the precision, postural, and visual constraints of the task were manipulated. Young children adopted a strategy where they first move their trunk toward the opening and then stabilize their trunk (freeze degrees of freedom) as the precision manual task is being performed. In contrast, adults and older children make compensatory trunk movements as the task is being performed. The 10-year-olds were similar to adults under the less constrained task conditions, but they resembled the 7-year-olds under the more challenging tasks. The ability to either suppress or allow postural fluctuations based on the constraints of a suprapostural task begins to develop at around 10 years of age. This ability, once developed, allows children to learn specific segmental movements required to complete a task within an environmental context.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2011

Manipulations of listeners’ echo perception are reflected in event-related potentials

Lisa D. Sanders; Benjamin H. Zobel; Richard L. Freyman; Rachel Keen

To gain information from complex auditory scenes, it is necessary to determine which of the many loudness, pitch, and timbre changes originate from a single source. Grouping sound into sources based on spatial information is complicated by reverberant energy bouncing off multiple surfaces and reaching the ears from directions other than the sources location. The ability to localize sounds despite these echoes has been explored with the precedence effect: Identical sounds presented from two locations with a short stimulus onset asynchrony (e.g., 1-5 ms) are perceived as a single source with a location dominated by the lead sound. Importantly, echo thresholds, the shortest onset asynchrony at which a listener reports hearing the lag sound as a separate source about half of the time, can be manipulated by presenting sound pairs in contexts. Event-related brain potentials elicited by physically identical sounds in contexts that resulted in listeners reporting either one or two sources were compared. Sound pairs perceived as two sources elicited a larger anterior negativity 100-250 ms after onset, previously termed the object-related negativity, and a larger posterior positivity 250-500 ms. These results indicate that the models of room acoustics listeners form based on recent experience with the spatiotemporal properties of sound modulate perceptual as well as later higher-level processing.

Collaboration


Dive into the Rachel Keen's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Neil E. Berthier

University of Massachusetts Amherst

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Richard L. Freyman

University of Massachusetts Amherst

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Michael E. McCarty

University of Massachusetts Amherst

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Heidi Kloos

University of Cincinnati

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kristin Shutts

University of Wisconsin-Madison

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Lisa D. Sanders

University of Massachusetts Amherst

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Yu-ping Chen

Georgia State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge