Regina M. Sullivan
University of California, Irvine
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Regina M. Sullivan.
Developmental Brain Research | 1986
Regina M. Sullivan; Michael Leon
During postnatal days 1-18, pups were simultaneously exposed to an odor and reinforcing tactile stimulation similar to that normally received from the dam. Control pups received only the odor, only the stimulation, or neither of these stimuli. On postnatal day 19, pups that had previously received simultaneous odor and tactile stimulation displayed both a behavioral odor preference and an enhanced 2-deoxyglucose uptake in specific olfactory bulb glomeruli to subsequent presentation of that odor. These results suggest that early olfactory learning enhances the neural response to odors that have acquired attractive value.
Developmental Brain Research | 1991
Regina M. Sullivan; James L. McGaugh; Michael Leon
The influence of norepinephrine (NE) on the acquisition of a conditioned odor preference and enhanced focal uptake of [14C]2-deoxyglucose (2-DG) within the olfactory bulb was assessed in neonatal rat pups. On postnatal day (PN) 6, pups were injected with either an NE receptor agonist (isoproterenol), NE receptor antagonist (propranolol or timolol), or saline before one-trial odor conditioning. The experimental conditioning group received a 10-min exposure to an odor (peppermint) and reinforcing tactile stimulation similar to that received from the dam. Control groups received only the odor, only the tactile stimulation, backwards presentation of the odor and tactile stimulation or neither of these stimuli. The next day, pups were either tested for an olfactory preference (Expts. 1 and 2) or assessed for differential olfactory bulb activity using the 2-DG technique (Expt. 3). The results indicate that early odor experience with either tactile stimulation or isoproterenol is sufficient to produce a learned behavioral preference and enhanced focal 2-DG uptake within the olfactory bulb. Moreover, an NE receptor blocker injected prior to training with odor and tactile stimulation blocks the acquisition of both behavioral preference and the enhanced 2-DG uptake. In Expt. 4, the effects of tactile stimulation and isoproterenol were further assessed. An odor paired with a moderate level of either of these stimuli produces learning. However, the simultaneous presentation of a moderate level of these stimuli paired with an odor does not result in an odor preference. An odor preference may be reinstated by simultaneous presentation of these stimuli, provided the level of each of these stimuli is too low to produce an odor preference when presented alone with an odor. These data suggest that exogenous NE and tactile stimulation are additive in their effect on learning. These results are discussed in terms of the neural mechanisms underlying reinforcement in infant rats.
Developmental Brain Research | 1985
Donald A. Wilson; Regina M. Sullivan; Michael Leon
Previous studies have shown that rat pups have an enhanced metabolic activity to familiar odors in specific glomeruli of the olfactory bulb. The present study examined extracellularly recorded mitral cell responses to odors in this glomerular region, in odor-familiar and odor-unfamiliar pups. Mitral cells in odor-familiar pups had significantly fewer excitatory and more inhibitory responses to the familiar odor than controls. There were no differences between groups in responses to a novel odor. These results demonstrate that neonatal exposure to odors selectively alters subsequent mitral cell responsiveness to that odor.
Developmental Brain Research | 1987
Regina M. Sullivan; Michael Leon
The expression of a conditioned odor preference and focal uptake of [14C]2-deoxyglucose (2-DG) within the olfactory bulb was assessed in neonatal rat pups that had undergone a single olfactory classical conditioning trial. At 6 days of age, rat pups were simultaneously exposed for 10 min to an odor (peppermint) and to a reinforcing tactile stimulation similar to that received from the dam. Three control groups received only the odor, only the stimulation, or neither of these stimuli. The next day, pups were either assessed for differential olfactory bulb activity using the 2-DG technique or tested for their olfactory preference behavior. Only pups that received simultaneous odor and tactile stimulation exhibited an attraction to the conditioned odor in the two-odor choice test. Furthermore, such pups had greater focal 2-DG uptake in the olfactory bulb glomeruli that were responsive to the odor than pups in all other groups. Thus, the olfactory bulb responds differentially to an odor which has acquired attractive value.
Developmental Brain Research | 1990
Regina M. Sullivan; Donald A. Wilson; Rebecca Wong; Adrian Correa; Michael Leon
Rat pups acquire an attraction for maternal odors, which can vary with maternal diet. In the two experiments reported here, maternal diet was modified and both pup behavioral responses and pup olfactory bulb neural responses [( 14C]2-DG uptake) to maternal odors were examined. In experiment 1, pups were reared from birth to postnatal day 19 with either a dam fed normal rat chow or a dam fed a sucrose-based diet which suppressed her normal maternal odor. In experiment 2, pups were raised from birth to postnatal day 19 with either a dam fed the sucrose-based diet adulterated with peppermint, or the non-scented sucrose-based diet. Pups selectively expressed both a behavioral attraction and an enhanced olfactory bulb neural response to odors that they experienced in the nest.
Physiology & Behavior | 1988
Regina M. Sullivan; Donald A. Wilson; Mi Haeng Kim; Michael Leon
Following olfactory classical conditioning, infant rats exhibit a preference for the conditioned odor and exhibit enhanced uptake of focal 14C 2-deoxyglucose (2-DG) within the olfactory bulb. The present experiments assessed the role of respiration on the expression of the enhanced 2-DG uptake response. Pups were conditioned from postnatal day (PN) 1-18 with an olfactory stimulus paired with a reinforcing tactile stimulus which mimics maternal contact (Odor-Stroke). Control pups received odor only or tactile stimulation only. On PN 19, pups received 1 of 3 tests: 1) a two-odor choice test, 2) an odor/2-DG test with normal respiration allowed, or 3) an odor/2-DG test with respiration experimentally controlled. The results indicated that: 1) Odor-Stroke pups learned the conditioned odor preference, 2) Odor-Stroke, normally respiring pups exhibited enhanced olfactory bulb 2-DG uptake when compared to control pups. No difference in respiration rate was detected between groups in normally respiring pups. 3) Odor Stroke pups whose breathing was experimentally controlled exhibited enhanced olfactory bulb 2-DG uptake when compared to control pups with an identical number of respirations. Together, these results demonstrate that modified respiration during testing is not required for the expression of a modified olfactory bulb response to learned attractive odors. Therefore, the data suggest that the olfactory system itself is modified by early learning.
The Journal of Neuroscience | 1989
Regina M. Sullivan; Donald A. Wilson; Michael Leon
The Journal of Neuroscience | 1987
Donald A. Wilson; Regina M. Sullivan; Michael Leon
Developmental Psychobiology | 1986
Regina M. Sullivan; Myron A. Hofer; Stephen C. Brake
Developmental Psychobiology | 1986
Regina M. Sullivan; Stephan C. Brake; Myron A. Hofer; Christina L. Williams