Renaud Laguesse
Université catholique de Louvain
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Renaud Laguesse.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology | 2017
Romina Palermo; Bruno Rossion; Gillian Rhodes; Renaud Laguesse; Tolga Tez; Bronwyn Hall; Andrea Albonico; Manuela Malaspina; Roberta Daini; Jessica Irons; Shahd Al-Janabi; Libby Taylor; Davide Rivolta; Elinor McKone
Diagnosis of developmental or congenital prosopagnosia (CP) involves self-report of everyday face recognition difficulties, which are corroborated with poor performance on behavioural tests. This approach requires accurate self-evaluation. We examine the extent to which typical adults have insight into their face recognition abilities across four experiments involving nearly 300 participants. The experiments used five tests of face recognition ability: two that tap into the ability to learn and recognize previously unfamiliar faces [the Cambridge Face Memory Test, CFMT; Duchaine, B., & Nakayama, K. (2006). The Cambridge Face Memory Test: Results for neurologically intact individuals and an investigation of its validity using inverted face stimuli and prosopagnosic participants. Neuropsychologia, 44(4), 576–585. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.07.001; and a newly devised test based on the CFMT but where the study phases involve watching short movies rather than viewing static faces—the CFMT-Films] and three that tap face matching [Benton Facial Recognition Test, BFRT; Benton, A., Sivan, A., Hamsher, K., Varney, N., & Spreen, O. (1983). Contribution to neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press; and two recently devised sequential face matching tests]. Self-reported ability was measured with the 15-item Kennerknecht et al. questionnaire [Kennerknecht, I., Ho, N. Y., & Wong, V. C. (2008). Prevalence of hereditary prosopagnosia (HPA) in Hong Kong Chinese population. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A, 146A(22), 2863–2870. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.32552]; two single-item questions assessing face recognition ability; and a new 77-item meta-cognition questionnaire. Overall, we find that adults with typical face recognition abilities have only modest insight into their ability to recognize faces on behavioural tests. In a fifth experiment, we assess self-reported face recognition ability in people with CP and find that some people who expect to perform poorly on behavioural tests of face recognition do indeed perform poorly. However, it is not yet clear whether individuals within this group of poor performers have greater levels of insight (i.e., into their degree of impairment) than those with more typical levels of performance.
Perception | 2013
Renaud Laguesse; Bruno Rossion
Compelling evidence that faces are perceived holistically or configurally comes from the composite face illusion: Identical top halves of a face are perceived as being different if they are aligned with different bottom halves. The visual illusion disappears when the top and bottom face halves are spatially misaligned. Whether this is because the two halves no longer form a whole face (ie they form two segmented parts), or because of an increase in interfeatures distance in the misaligned condition (eg eyes-mouth distance) remains unclear. Here, thirty-four participants performed a delayed matching composite task in which the amount of spatial misalignment between face halves varied parametrically (from 8.33% of face width to 100%). The difference in performance between aligned and misaligned faces (ie the composite face effect) was already of full magnitude at the smallest level of misalignment. These results imply that a small spatial misalignment is sufficient to measure the composite face effect. From a theoretical standpoint, they indicate that it is the breaking of a whole configuration rather than the increase in relative distance between the face parts that explains the presence or absence of the composite face effect, clarifying an outstanding issue concerning the nature of holistic face perception.
Behavioural Neurology | 2010
Goedele Van Belle; Philippe Lefèvre; Renaud Laguesse; Thomas Busigny; Peter De Graef; Karl Verfaillie; Bruno Rossion
How familiar and unfamiliar faces are perceived remains largely unknown. Two views have dominated this field of research. On the one hand, recordings of eye fixations on faces [10] and response classification experiments [2] suggest that a face is processed in terms of its individual components, or facial features (mouth, eyes, nose, . . . ), a strategy called analytical processing. On the other hand, there is strong behavioral evidence for interdependence in the processing of different features of a face [6,7], rather supporting holistic processing of the face [7]. According to the latter holistic view, facial features are simultaneously perceived and integrated into a single representation, so that the perceptual field is that of the whole face. To shed light on this issue, in two recent studies, we recorded eye movements in a neurological patient [5] suffering from a selective impairment in face recognition (acquired prosopagnosia). Previously, we showed that (1) PS fixates exactly on each of the main features of the face (mouth, left eye, right eye), contrary to normal observers who fixate mainly centrally on the top of the nose, around the geometric centre of the face [3]
Visual Cognition | 2012
Susanne Quadflieg; Alexander Todorov; Renaud Laguesse; Bruno Rossion
Initial evidence indicates that face-based judgements of socially relevant characteristics such as peoples trustworthiness or attractiveness are linked to the configural/holistic processing of facial cues. What remains a matter of debate, however, is whether such processing is actually necessary for normal social judgements to occur and whether it resembles the type of integrative processing as required for facial identification. To address these issues, we asked a well-characterized case of acquired prosopagnosia (PS) with a marked deficit in holistic processing for face identity to rate a series of faces on several dimensions of social relevance. PS provided ratings within the normal range for most of the social characteristics probed (i.e., aggression, attractiveness, confidence, intelligence, sociability, trustworthiness). Her evaluations deviated from those of healthy controls only when facial dominance was concerned. Taken together, these data demonstrate that the inability to integrate facial information during face individuation does not necessarily translate into a generalized deficit to evaluate faces on social dimensions.
Visual Cognition | 2016
Quoc C. Vuong; Verena Willenbockel; Friederike G. S. Zimmermann; Aliette Lochy; Renaud Laguesse; Adam Dryden; Bruno Rossion
ABSTRACT There is a view that faces and objects are processed by different brain mechanisms. Different factors may modulate the extent to which face mechanisms are used for objects. To distinguish these factors, we present a new parametric multipart three-dimensional object set that provides researchers with a rich degree of control of important features for visual recognition such as individual parts and the spatial configuration of those parts. All other properties being equal, we demonstrate that perceived facelikeness in terms of spatial configuration facilitated performance at matching individual exemplars of the new object set across viewpoint changes (Experiment 1). Importantly, facelikeness did not affect perceptual discriminability (Experiment 2) or similarity (Experiment 3). Our findings suggest that perceptual resemblance to faces based on spatial configuration of parts is important for visual recognition even after equating physical and perceptual similarity. Furthermore, the large parametrically controlled object set and the standardized procedures to generate additional exemplars will provide the research community with invaluable tools to further understand visual recognition and visual learning.
Journal of Vision | 2012
Renaud Laguesse; Giulia Dormal; Aurélie Biervoye; Dana Kuefner; Bruno Rossion
Journal of Vision | 2013
Renaud Laguesse; Tolga Tez; Bronwyn Hall; Jessica Irons; Elinor McKone; Roberta Daini; Andrea Albonico; Manuela Malaspina; Elisabeth Taylor; Gillian Rhodes; Alexandra Charpentier; Bruno Rossion; Romina Palermo
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | 2018
Aliette Lochy; Friederike G. S. Zimmermann; Renaud Laguesse; Verena Willenbockel; Bruno Rossion; Quoc C. Vuong
Journal of Vision | 2011
Renaud Laguesse; Bruno Rossion
Journal of Vision | 2015
Aliette Lochy; Renaud Laguesse; Friederike Zimmermann; Verena Willenbockel; Bruno Rossion; Quoc C. Vuong