Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Robert I. Davis is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Robert I. Davis.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1993

Complex noise exposures: An energy analysis

William A. Ahroon; Roger P. Hamernik; Robert I. Davis

Industrial noise environments usually present a complex stimulus to the exposed individual. These environments often contain mixtures of multiply reflected impact noises and a relatively Gaussian broadband noise. Noise exposure standards do not consider the possibility of interactions between the two classes of noise that can exacerbate the amount of hearing trauma. This paper presents the results of a large series of experiments designed to document the hazard posed to hearing from complex noise exposures. Twenty-three groups of chinchillas with 5 to 11 animals per group (total N = 135) were exposed for 5 days to either octave bands of noise, impacts alone, or combinations of impact and octave bands of noise. Evoked potential measures of hearing thresholds and cochleograms were used to quantify the noise-induced trauma. The results show that, for sound exposure levels (SEL) which produce less than approximately 10 dB PTS (permanent threshold shift) or 5% total sensory cell loss, equal-energy exposures tend to produce equivalent effects on hearing. However, there is a range of at least 10 dB in the SEL parameter where hearing loss from equal-energy exposures at a particular SEL can be exacerbated by increasing the repetition rate of the impacts or by the addition of a Gaussian low-level noise. The exacerbation of trauma from the addition of a Gaussian continuous noise is dependent upon the spectrum of that noise.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1993

Audiometric and histological differences between the effects of continuous and impulsive noise exposures

Roger P. Hamernik; William A. Ahroon; Keng D. Hsueh; Sheau Fang Lei; Robert I. Davis

An experiment was designed to determine if, for equal SPL and power spectrum, the effects on hearing of high-kurtosis noise exposures and a Gaussian noise exposure are different and the extent to which any differences measured in terms of audiometric and histological variables are frequency specific. Three groups of chinchillas with 10 animals/group were exposed for 5 days at 90 dB SPL to one of three types of noise, each with the same power spectrum. The impulsiveness, defined by the kurtosis, and the region of the spectrum from which the impulsive components of the noise were created differed for two of the noises, while the third was a continuous Gaussian noise. The results show that the most impulsive noise produced up to 20 dB greater permanent threshold shift at the high frequencies than did the Gaussian noise exposure. However, these audiometric results were difficult to reconcile with the pattern of sensory cell losses that showed statistically significant larger losses of outer hair cells for the impulsive exposure in the 0.25-kHz region. When the impacts in a high-kurtosis noise were created from the energy in the 1- through 6-kHz region of the spectrum, the audiometric profile of hearing loss was similar to that produced by the Gaussian noise; however, inner hair cell losses were significantly greater in the 4-kHz octave band region of the cochlea.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1992

Hearing threshold shifts from repeated 6‐h daily exposure to impact noise

Roger P. Hamernik; William A. Ahroon; Robert I. Davis; Sheau Fang Lei

Exposure of chinchillas to broadband, high-level impact noise on an interrupted 6-h daily schedule over 20 days has shown that pure-tone thresholds measured immediately following each daily exposure improve as much as 30 dB despite the continuing noise exposure. The time constant of this recovery effect (toughening) and the magnitude of the effect are related to the audiometric test frequency and the exposure energy. The trauma, quantified by permanent threshold shifts and sensory cell losses, produced by the interrupted exposure paradigm is generally less than that produced by an equal-energy uninterrupted exposure. The wide variations in the temporal pattern of threshold shift across similarly exposed animals suggest that the toughening effect reflects the underlying susceptibility of that animal to noise trauma.


Ear and Hearing | 2010

Application of the kurtosis statistic to the evaluation of the risk of hearing loss in workers exposed to high-level complex noise.

Yiming Zhao; Wei Qiu; Lin Zeng; Shan-song Chen; Xiaoru Cheng; Robert I. Davis; Roger P. Hamernik

Objective: Develop dose-response relations for two groups of industrial workers exposed to Gaussian or non-Gaussian (complex) types of continuous noises and to investigate what role, if any, the kurtosis statistic can play in the evaluation of industrial noise-induced hearing loss (NIHL). Design: Audiometric and noise exposure data were acquired on a population (N = 195) of screened workers from a textile manufacturing plant and a metal fabrication facility located in Henan province of China. Thirty-two of the subjects were exposed to non-Gaussian (non-G) noise and 163 were exposed to a Gaussian (G) continuous noise. Each subject was given a general physical and an otologic examination. Hearing threshold levels (0.5–8.0 kHz) were age adjusted (ISO-1999) and the prevalence of NIHL at 3, 4, or 6 kHz was determined. The kurtosis metric, which is sensitive to the peak and temporal characteristics of a noise, was introduced into the calculation of the cumulative noise exposure metric. Using the prevalence of hearing loss and the cumulative noise exposure metric, a dose-response relation for the G and non-G noise-exposed groups was constructed. Results: An analysis of the noise environments in the two plants showed that the noise exposures in the textile plant were of a Gaussian type with an Leq(A)8hr that varied from 96 to 105 dB whereas the exposures in the metal fabrication facility with an Leq(A)8hr = 95 dB were of a non-G type containing high levels (up to 125 dB peak SPL) of impact noise. The kurtosis statistic was used to quantify the deviation of the non-G noise environment from the Gaussian. The dose-response relation for the non-G noise-exposed subjects showed a higher prevalence of hearing loss for a comparable cumulative noise exposure than did the G noise-exposed subjects. By introducing the kurtosis variable into the temporal component of the cumulative noise exposure calculation, the two dose-response curves could be made to overlap, essentially yielding an equivalent noise-induced effect for the two study groups. Conclusions: For the same exposure level, the prevalence of NIHL is greater in workers exposed to non-G noise environments than for workers exposed to G noise. The kurtosis metric may be a reasonable candidate for use in modifying exposure level calculations that are used to estimate the risk of NIHL from any type of noise exposure environment. However, studies involving a large number of workers with well-documented exposures are needed before a relation between a metric such as the kurtosis and the risk of hearing loss can be refined.


Ear and Hearing | 2009

Role of the kurtosis statistic in evaluating complex noise exposures for the protection of hearing.

Robert I. Davis; Wei Qiu; Roger P. Hamernik

Objective: To highlight a selection of data that illustrate the need for better descriptors of complex industrial noise environments for use in the protection of hearing. Design: The data were derived using a chinchilla model. All noise exposures had the same total energy and the same spectrum; that is, they were equal energy exposures presented at an overall 100 dB(A) SPL that differed only in the scheduling of the exposure and the value of the kurtosis, &bgr;(t), a statistical metric. Hearing thresholds were determined before and after noise exposure using the auditory-evoked potential measured from the inferior colliculus in the brain stem. Cochlear damage was estimated from sensory-cell counts (cochleograms). Results: (1) For equivalent energy and spectra, exposure to a high-kurtosis, non-Gaussian noise produced substantially greater hearing and sensory-cell loss in the chinchilla model than a low-kurtosis, Gaussian noise. (2) &bgr;(t) computed on the amplitude distribution of the noise could clearly differentiate between the effects of Gaussian and non-Gaussian noise environments. (3) &bgr;(t) can order the extent of the trauma as determined by hearing thresholds and sensory-cell loss. Conclusions: The noise level in combination with the statistical properties of the noise quantified by &bgr;(t) clearly differentiate the effects between both continuous and interrupted and intermittent Gaussian and non-Gaussian noise environments. For the same energy and spectrum, the non-Gaussian environments are clearly the more hazardous. The use of both an energy and kurtosis metric can better predict the hazard of a high-level complex noise than the use of an energy metric alone (as is the current practice). These results point out the need for a new approach to the analysis and quantification of industrial noise for the purpose of hearing conservation practice.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1989

Noise and vibration interactions: Effects on hearing

Roger P. Hamernik; William A. Ahroon; Robert I. Davis; Alf Axelsson

There is the suggestion in the literature that vibration may potentiate the effects of noise and may thus increase the risk of hearing loss in a variety of exposure situations. However, in human experimental studies, which, by necessity, are limited to low levels of exposure, the effects measured are relatively small. A very limited number of animal studies have also shown an enhanced noise-induced hearing loss in the presence of vibration, but the scope of these studies is limited. The animal studies (chinchilla) that form the basis of this report were performed using a 30-Hz, 3g rms and a 20-Hz, 1.3g rms cage vibration separately and in combination with continuous noise (95-dB, 0.5-kHz octave band) and impact noise (113, 119, or 125 dB peak SPL) exposure paradigms. All exposures lasted for 5 days. The impact noise exposures were designed to have approximately equal total energy. Temporary and permanent threshold shifts were measured using evoked potentials, and sensory cell loss was measured using surface preparation histology. The results obtained from some of the noise/vibration paradigms showed that such exposures can alter some of the dependent measures of hearing. This effect was statistically significant only for the stronger vibration exposure conditions and was evident primarily in the extent of the outer hair cell losses and in the shape of the PTS audiogram.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2013

The value of a kurtosis metric in estimating the hazard to hearing of complex industrial noise exposures

Wei Qiu; Roger P. Hamernik; Robert I. Davis

A series of Gaussian and non-Gaussian equal energy noise exposures were designed with the objective of establishing the extent to which the kurtosis statistic could be used to grade the severity of noise trauma produced by the exposures. Here, 225 chinchillas distributed in 29 groups, with 6 to 8 animals per group, were exposed at 97 dB SPL. The equal energy exposures were presented either continuously for 5 d or on an interrupted schedule for 19 d. The non-Gaussian noises all differed in the level of the kurtosis statistic or in the temporal structure of the noise, where the latter was defined by different peak, interval, and duration histograms of the impact noise transients embedded in the noise signal. Noise-induced trauma was estimated from auditory evoked potential hearing thresholds and surface preparation histology that quantified sensory cell loss. Results indicated that the equal energy hypothesis is a valid unifying principle for estimating the consequences of an exposure if and only if the equivalent energy exposures had the same kurtosis. Furthermore, for the same level of kurtosis the detailed temporal structure of an exposure does not have a strong effect on trauma.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1988

The influence of whole‐body vibration on noise‐induced hearing loss: A review of animal experiments

Roger P. Hamernik; William A. Ahroon; Robert I. Davis; Donald Henderson

There is the suggestion in the literature that vibration may potentiate the effects of noise and may pose an increased risk of hearing loss. However, in human experimental studies, which, by necessity, are limited to low levels of TTS, the effects measured are consistent but relatively small. A very limited number of animal studies have also shown an enhanced hearing loss, but the scope of these studies is limited by a large intersubject variability and a small number of subjects. Also, the high levels of stimulation that were used in some of these animal experiments were not realistic. Our recent animal studies (chinchilla) have used a 30 Hz, 3‐g‐rms cage vibration in combination with continuous noise (95‐dB, 0.5‐kHz octave band) and impact noise (113‐, 119‐, or 125‐dB peak SPL) exposure paradigms. All exposures lasted for 5 days. The impact noise exposures were designed to have an equal total energy. Temporary (compound) and permanent threshold shifts were measured using evoked potentials. Sensory cell ...


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2006

Noise‐induced hearing loss from non‐Gaussian equal energy exposures

Robert I. Davis; Wei Qiu; Roger P. Hamernik

Data from several different exposures [Hamernik et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 114, 386–395 (2003)] showed that, for equivalent energy [Leq=100 dB(A)] and spectra, exposure to a continuous, non‐Gaussian (nonG) noise produces greater hearing and sensory cell loss in the chinchilla than a Gaussian (G) noise. The statistical metric, kurtosis, could order the extent of the trauma. We extend these results to Leq=90 and 110 dB(A), non‐Gaussian noises generated using broadband noise bursts, and band‐limited impacts within a continuous G background noise. Data from nine new experimental groups with 11 or 12 chinchillas/group will be presented. Evoked response audiometry established hearing thresholds and surface preparation histology quantified sensory cell loss. There were clear intensity‐related effects. At the lowest levels there were no differences in the trauma produced by G and nonG exposures. At Leq=90 dB(A), nonG exposures produced increased trauma relative to equivalent G exposures. By removing energy from the impulsive transients by limiting their bandwidth, trauma could be reduced. The use of noise bursts to produce the nonG noise also reduced the amount of trauma. A metric based on kurtosis and energy may be useful in modifying existing exposure criteria. [Work supported by NIOSH.]


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1994

The effects of primed and interrupted noise exposure paradigms on hearing loss

Roberto M; William A. Ahroon; Roger P. Hamernik; Robert I. Davis; Christopher J. Case

Exposure to low‐level noise (priming) produces a protective effect on the auditory system evidenced by a reduced permanent threshold shift (PTS) from a subsequent high‐level exposure. Similarly, regularly interrupting a high‐level noise exposure causes the threshold shift following daily repeated exposures to the noise to be reduced with a concomitant lowering of PTS. These effects, collectively referred to as ‘‘toughening,’’ can exceed 35 dB. If the toughening mechanism elicited during both types of exposure paradigms has a common origin, then combining the paradigms should yield predictable results. Results from nine exposure paradigms with five to six chinchillas in each paradigm are reported. Impacts of 113 or 119 dB peak SPL and pink noise of 72 or 78 dB SPL presented over five or 20 days in an interrupted or noninterrupted paradigm were used. Results suggest that priming can affect the PTS but has little or no effect on the toughening produced during interrupted exposures or the asymptotic threshold...

Collaboration


Dive into the Robert I. Davis's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Roger P. Hamernik

State University of New York at Plattsburgh

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

William A. Ahroon

University of Texas at Dallas

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Wei Qiu

State University of New York System

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Sheau Fang Lei

National Cheng Kung University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

George Turrentine

University of Texas at Dallas

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Roberto M

University of Texas at Dallas

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge