Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Robin A. Murphy is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Robin A. Murphy.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: General | 2005

Depressive Realism and Outcome Density Bias in Contingency Judgments: The Effect of the Context and Intertrial Interval

Rachel M. Msetfi; Robin A. Murphy; Jane Simpson; Diana Kornbrot

The perception of the effectiveness of instrumental actions is influenced by depressed mood. Depressive realism (DR) is the claim that depressed people are particularly accurate in evaluating instrumentality. In two experiments, the authors tested the DR hypothesis using an action-outcome contingency judgment task. DR effects were a function of intertrial interval length and outcome density, suggesting that depressed mood is accompanied by reduced contextual processing rather than increased judgment accuracy. The DR effect was observed only when participants were exposed to extended periods in which no actions or outcomes occurred. This implies that DR may result from an impairment in contextual processing rather than accurate but negative expectations. Therefore, DR is consistent with a cognitive distortion view of depression.


Psychology of Learning and Motivation | 1996

Associative and Normative Models of Causal Induction: Reacting to Versus Understanding Cause

A. G. Baker; Robin A. Murphy; Frédéric Vallée-Tourangeau

This chapter discusses two views of causal judgment that are roughly analogous to a distinction between being able to react appropriately to causes and being able to understand them. The associationist view is identified with the British Empiricists. It claims that the judgments of cause come from certain empirical cues to causality, which includes: (1) regular succession, (2) temporal contiguity, and (3) spatial contiguity. Associations between events are strengthened when the events are contiguous and are weakened when an event occurs by itself. These models have the advantage that they are computationally simple and they impose a low memory load on the organism because experience is stored as a small number of associative strengths. They have the disadvantage that information about past events is lost in the computation. Also, these models do not have episodic memory. The second classes of models are referred to as normative models. They claim that humans and other animals compute the covariation between cause and effect and then use this information as part of a causal model or schema. The chapter reviews that a retrospective normative model makes the choice of domain in which to do the normative calculation. Associative models and those that involve causal models or schema are appropriate to overlapping but not identical domains of information processing. Simple associative ideas can be used in many situations in which contiguity is important, but in which mental models are unavailable. These can be used in situations in which associative networks are difficult to apply.


Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology | 1998

Judging the importance of constant and variable candidate causes: A test of the power PC theory.

Frédéric Vallée-Tourangeau; Robin A. Murphy; Susan Drew; A. G. Baker

In two causal induction experiments subjects rated the importance of pairs of candidate causes in the production of a target effect; one candidate was present on every trial (constant cause), whereas the other was present on only some trials (variable cause). The design of both experiments consisted of a factorial combination of two values of the variable causes covariation with the effect and three levels of the base rate of the effect. Judgements of the constant cause were inversely proportional to the level of covariation of the variable cause but were proportional to the base rate of the effect. The judgements were consistent with the predictions derived from the Rescorla-Wagner (1972) model of associative learning and with the predictions of the causal power theory of the probabilistic contrast model (Cheng, 1997) or “power PC theory”. However, judgements of the importance of the variable candidate cause were proportional to the base rate of the effect, a phenomenon that is in some cases anticipated by the power PC theory. An alternative associative model, Pearces (1987) similarity-based generalization model, predicts the influence of the base rate of the effect on the estimates of both the constant and the variable cause.


Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology | 2007

Depressive realism and the effect of intertrial interval on judgements of zero, positive, and negative contingencies

Rachel M. Msetfi; Robin A. Murphy; Jane Simpson

In three experiments we tested how the spacing of trials during acquisition of zero, positive, and negative response–outcome contingencies differentially affected depressed and nondepressed students’ judgements. Experiment 1 found that nondepressed participants’ judgements of zero contingencies increased with longer intertrial intervals (ITIs) but not simply longer procedure durations. Depressed groups’ judgements were not sensitive to either manipulation, producing an effect known as depressive realism only with long ITIs. Experiments 2 and 3 tested predictions of Chengs (1997) Power PC theory and the Rescorla–Wagner (1972) model, that the increase in context exposure experienced during the ITI might influence judgements most with negative contingencies and least with positive contingencies. Results suggested that depressed people were less sensitive to differences in contingency and contextual exposure. We propose that a context-processing difference between depressed and nondepressed people removes any objective notion of “realism” that was originally employed to explain the depressive realism effect (Alloy & Abramson, 1979).


Journal of Experimental Psychology: General | 2010

Stereotype formation: biased by association.

Mike E. Le Pelley; Stian Reimers; Guglielmo Calvini; Russell Spears; Tom Beesley; Robin A. Murphy

We propose that biases in attitude and stereotype formation might arise as a result of learned differences in the extent to which social groups have previously been predictive of behavioral or physical properties. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrate that differences in the experienced predictiveness of groups with respect to evaluatively neutral information influence the extent to which participants later form attitudes and stereotypes about those groups. In contrast, Experiment 3 shows no influence of predictiveness when using a procedure designed to emphasize the use of higher level reasoning processes, a finding consistent with the idea that the root of the predictiveness bias is not in reasoning. Experiments 4 and 5 demonstrate that the predictiveness bias in formation of group beliefs does not depend on participants making global evaluations of groups. These results are discussed in relation to the associative mechanisms proposed by Mackintosh (1975) to explain similar phenomena in animal conditioning and associative learning.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes | 2004

A role for CS-US contingency in Pavlovian conditioning

Robin A. Murphy; A. G. Baker

Two experiments evaluated the role of conditioned stimulus-unconditioned stimulus (CS-US) contingency in appetitive Pavlovian conditioning in rats. In both experiments, some groups received a positively contingent CS signaling an increased likelihood of the US relative to the absence of the CS. These groups were compared with control treatments in which the likelihood of the US was the same in the presence and absence of the CS. A trial marker served as a trial context. Experiment 1 found contingency sensitivity. There was a reciprocal relationship between responding to the CS and the trial marker. Experiment 2 showed that this result was not stimulus or response specific. These results are consistent with associative explanations and the idea that rats are sensitive to CS-US contingency.


Memory & Cognition | 2000

Asymptotic judgment of cause in a relative validity paradigm

A. G. Baker; Frédéric Vallée-Tourangeau; Robin A. Murphy

We report three experiments in which we tested asymptotic and dynamic predictions of the Rescorla—Wagner (R—W) model and the asymptotic predictions of Cheng’s probabilistic contrast model (PCM) concerning judgments of causality when there are two possible causal candidates. We used a paradigm in which the presence of a causal candidate that is highly correlated with an effect influences judgments of a second, moderately correlated or uncorrelated cause. In Experiment 1, which involved a moderate outcome density, judgments of a moderately positive cause were attenuated when it was paired with either a perfect positive or perfect negative cause. This attenuation was robust over a large set of trials but was greater when the strong predictor was positive. In Experiment 2, in which there was a low overall density of outcomes, judgments of a moderately correlated positive cause were elevated when this cause was paired with a perfect negative causal candidate. This elevation was also quite robust over a large set of trials. In Experiment 3, estimates of the strength of a causal candidate that was uncorrelated with the outcome were reduced when it was paired with a perfect cause. The predictions of three theoretical models of causal judgments are considered. Both the R-W model and Cheng’s PCM accounted for some but not all aspects of the data. Pearce’s model of stimulus generalization accounts for a greater proportion of the data.


Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology | 2012

The effect of mild depression on time discrimination

Rachel M. Msetfi; Robin A. Murphy; Diana Kornbrot

Depressed mood states affect subjective perceptions of time but it is not clear whether this is due to changes in the underlying timing mechanisms, such as the speed of the internal clock. In order to study depression effects on time perception, two experiments using time discrimination methods with short (<300 ms) and long (>1,000 ms) durations were conducted. Student participants who were categorized as mildly depressed by their scores on the Beck Depression Inventory were less able than controls to discriminate between two longer durations but were equally able to discriminate shorter intervals. The results suggest that mildly depressed or dysphoric moods do not affect pacemaker speed. It is more likely that depression affects the ability to maintain attention to elapsing duration.


Psychopharmacology | 2011

5-HT modulation by acute tryptophan depletion of human instrumental contingency judgements

Henry W. Chase; Molly J. Crockett; Rachel M. Msetfi; Robin A. Murphy; Luke Clark; Barbara J. Sahakian; Trevor W. Robbins

IntroductionThe concept of ‘depressive realism’, that depression leads to more accurate perception of causal control, has been influential in the field of depression research, but remains controversial. Recent work testing contingency learning has suggested that contextual processing might determine realism-like effects. Serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, (5-HT)), which is implicated in the pathophysiology of depression, might also influence contextual processing. Using acute tryptophan depletion (ATD), we tested the hypothesis that dysfunctional serotoninergic neurotransmission influences contingency judgements in dysphoric subjects via an effect on contextual processing.Materials and methodsWe employed a novel contingency learning task to obtain separate measures (ratings) of the causal effect of partcipants’ responses and efficacy of the background context over an outcome. Participants, without a history of depression, completed this task on and off ATD in a double-blind, placebo-controlled, within-subjects design.ResultsAs with other work on contingency learning, the effects of ATD were related to baseline mood levels. Although no overall effects of ATD were observed, the subgroup of participants with low Beck depression inventory (BDI) scores showed reduced ratings of contextual control and improved accuracy of contingency judgements under positive contingencies following ATD, compared to placebo. High BDI participants demonstrated low accuracy in contingency judgements, regardless of serotoninergic status.ConclusionsNo effect of ATD on contingency judgements was observed in the group as a whole, but effects were observed in a subgroup of participants with low BDI scores. We discuss these data in light of the context processing hypothesis, and prior research on 5-HT and depressive realism.


Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B-comparative and Physiological Psychology | 2003

Learned irrelevance and retrospective correlation learning

A. G. Baker; Robin A. Murphy; Rick Mehta

In 1973 Mackintosh reported an interference effect that he called learned irrelevance in which exposure to uncorrelated (CS/US) presentation of the unconditional stimulus (US) and the conditioned stimulus (CS) interfered with future Pavlovian conditioning. It has been argued that there is no specific interference effect in learned irrelevance; rather the interference is the sum of independent CS and US exposure effects (CS + US). We review previous research on this question and report two new experiments. We conclude that learned irrelevance is a consequence of a contingency learning and a specific learned irrelevance mechanism. Moreover even the “independent exposure controls”, used in previous experiments to support the CS and US exposure account, provide support for the correlation learning process.

Collaboration


Dive into the Robin A. Murphy's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Diana Kornbrot

University of Hertfordshire

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge