Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Russell M. Bauer is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Russell M. Bauer.


Brain and Cognition | 1985

Processing of faces by patients with unilateral hemisphere lesions: I. Dissociation between judgments of facial affect and facial identity

Dawn Bowers; Russell M. Bauer; H. Branch Coslett; Kenneth M. Heilman

In this study, right-hemisphere-damaged (RHD) subjects performed significantly worse than LHD and NHD controls across a series of seven facial identity and facial affect tasks. Even when the patient groups were statistically equated on a measure of visuoperceptual ability, the RHD group remained impaired on three emotional tasks--naming, picking, and discriminating emotional faces. These findings suggest that the defects shown by RHD patients on facial affect tasks cannot be solely attributed to defects in visuoperceptual processing and that the right-hemisphere superiority for processing facial affect exists above and beyond its superiority for processing facial identity.


Brain and Language | 1999

Conduction Aphasia and the Arcuate Fasciculus: A Reexamination of the Wernicke–Geschwind Model ☆

Jeffrey M. Anderson; Robin L. Gilmore; Bruce Crosson; Russell M. Bauer; Stephen E. Nadeau; D.Q. Beversdorf; Jean E. Cibula; M. Rogish; S. Kortencamp; J.D. Hughes; L.J. Gonzalez Rothi; Kenneth M. Heilman

Wernicke, and later Geschwind, posited that the critical lesion in conduction aphasia is in the dominant hemispheres arcuate fasciculus. This white matter pathway was thought to connect the anterior language production areas with the posterior language areas that contain auditory memories of words (a phonological lexicon). Alternatively, conduction aphasia might be induced by cortical dysfunction, which impairs the phonological output lexicon. We observed an epileptic patient who, during cortical stimulation of her posterior superior temporal gyrus, demonstrated frequent phonemic paraphasias, decreased repetition of words, and yet had intact semantic knowledge, a pattern consistent with conduction aphasia. These findings suggest that cortical dysfunction alone may induce conduction aphasia.


Journal of The International Neuropsychological Society | 2003

Left and right basal ganglia and frontal activity during language generation: Contributions to lexical, semantic, and phonological processes

Bruce Crosson; Hope Benefield; M. Allison Cato; Joseph Sadek; Anna Bacon Moore; Christina E. Wierenga; Kaundinya S. Gopinath; David Soltysik; Russell M. Bauer; Edward J. Auerbach; Didem Gökçay; Christiana M. Leonard; Richard W. Briggs

fMRI was used to determine the frontal, basal ganglia, and thalamic structures engaged by three facets of language generation: lexical status of generated items, the use of semantic vs. phonological information during language generation, and rate of generation. During fMRI, 21 neurologically normal subjects performed four tasks: generation of nonsense syllables given beginning and ending consonant blends, generation of words given a rhyming word, generation of words given a semantic category at a fast rate (matched to the rate of nonsense syllable generation), and generation of words given a semantic category at a slow rate (matched to the rate of generating of rhyming words). Components of a left pre-SMA-dorsal caudate nucleus-ventral anterior thalamic loop were active during word generation from rhyming or category cues but not during nonsense syllable generation. Findings indicate that this loop is involved in retrieving words from pre-existing lexical stores. Relatively diffuse activity in the right basal ganglia (caudate nucleus and putamen) also was found during word-generation tasks but not during nonsense syllable generation. Given the relative absence of right frontal activity during the word generation tasks, we suggest that the right basal ganglia activity serves to suppress right frontal activity, preventing right frontal structures from interfering with language production. Current findings establish roles for the left and the right basal ganglia in word generation. Hypotheses are discussed for future research to help refine our understanding of basal ganglia functions in language generation.


Neuropsychologia | 1987

Comprehension of emotional prosody following unilateral hemispheric lesions: Processing defect versus distraction defect ☆

Dawn Bowers; H. Branch Coslett; Russell M. Bauer; Lynn J. Speedie; Kenneth M. Heilman

Two studies were conducted in order to determine whether the poor performance of RHD patients on emotional prosody tasks could be attributed to a defect in perceiving/categorizing emotional prosody (processing defect) or to a problem in being distracted by the semantic content of affectively intoned sentences (distraction defect). In one study, patients with RHD, LHD or NHD listened to affectively intoned sentences in which the semantic content was congruent or incongruent with the emotional prosody. In a second study, the patients listened to affectively intoned sentences that had been speech filtered or unfiltered. Findings from these studies indicate that both processing and distraction defects are present in RHD patients.


Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology | 1986

Nocturnal hypoxia and Neuropsychological variables

David T. R. Berry; Wilse B. Webb; A. J. Block; Russell M. Bauer; Daniel A. Switzer

Hypoxia is a well known cause of brain dysfunction. Neuropsychological impairments have been observed in normal subjects experiencing hypoxia iatrogenically as well as in patients with chronic lung disease. Recent investigations have demonstrated significant nocturnal hypoxia in subjects with sleep-disordered breathing. In the present study, heavy-snoring males, a group known to experience frequent episodes of sleep-disordered breathing received neuropsychological testing and a night of continuous monitoring of respiratory parameters. Partial correlations, controlling for age, weight, and education, indicated reliable relationships between nocturnal hypoxia and measures of general intelligence, verbal and nonverbal memory, and expressive verbal fluency. It is proposed that heavy-snoring males may potentially serve as a population in which to model the neurobehavioral effects of hypoxia. Further research in subjects with sleep-disordered breathing may help clarify the extent of the possible cognitive deficits as well as point out possible ameliorative treatments.


Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | 2004

Processing Words with Emotional Connotation: An fMRI Study of Time Course and Laterality in Rostral Frontal and Retrosplenial Cortices

M. Allison Cato; Bruce Crosson; Didem Gokcay; David Soltysik; Christina E. Wierenga; Kaundinya S. Gopinath; Nathan Himes; Heather Belanger; Russell M. Bauer; Ira Fischler; Leslie J. Gonzalez-Rothi; Richard W. Briggs

Responses of rostral frontal and retrosplenial cortices to the emotional significance of words were measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Twenty-six strongly righthanded participants engaged in a language task that alternated between silent word generation to categories with positive, negative, or neutral emotional connotation and a baseline task of silent repetition of emotionally neutral words. Activation uniquely associated with word generation to categories with positive or negative versus neutral emotional connotation occurred bilaterally in rostral frontal and retrosplenial cortices. Furthermore, the time courses of activity in these areas differed, indicating that they subserve different functions in processing the emotional connotation of words. Namely, the retrosplenial cortex appears to be involved in evaluating the emotional salience of information from external sources, whereas the rostral frontal cortex also plays a role in internal generation of words with emotional connotation. In both areas, activity associated with positive or negative emotional connotation was more extensive in the left hemisphere than the right, regardless of valence, presumably due to the language demands of word generation. The present findings localize specific areas in the brain that are involved in processing emotional meaning of words within the brains distributed semantic system. In addition, time course analysis reveals diverging mechanisms in anterior and posterior cortical areas during processing of words with emotional significance.


Neuropsychologia | 2002

Category-specific naming deficits for objects and actions: semantic attribute and grammatical role hypotheses

Lisa H Lu; Bruce Crosson; Stephen E. Nadeau; Kenneth M. Heilman; Leslie J. Gonzalez-Rothi; Anastasia M. Raymer; Robin L. Gilmore; Russell M. Bauer

Research on category-specific naming deficits and on noun and verb naming has raised questions about how organization of knowledge in the brain impacts word retrieval. The semantic attribute hypothesis posits that lexical access is mediated by brain systems that process the most definitive attributes of specific concepts. For example, it has been suggested that the most definitive attribute of living things is their visual form, whereas the most definitive attribute of non-living things is their function. The competing grammatical role hypothesis posits that access to a word depends on the grammatical role it plays in a sentence. Since nouns and verbs have different roles, it is assumed that the brain uses different systems to process them. These two hypotheses were tested in experimental subjects who had undergone left anterior temporal lobectomy (LATL) or right anterior temporal lobectomy (RATL) by assessing confrontation naming of living things, tools/implements, non-human actions, and human actions. The names of living things and implements are nouns and the names of actions are verbs. Within each grammatical class, items were characterized either predominantly by visual attributes (living things and non-human actions) or by attributes related to human activity (implements and human actions). Our results support the semantic attribute hypothesis. Patients with LATL were worse at naming tools/implements and human actions than RATL patients. Dysfunction in or removal of the left anterior temporal lobe disrupts fronto-temporal connections from the uncinate fasciculus. These connections may mediate activation of action-related information (i.e. movement plan and/or motor use) that facilitates the retrieval of names for tools/implements and human actions.


Neurology | 2010

Testing the limits: Cautions and concerns regarding the new Wechsler IQ and Memory scales

David W. Loring; Russell M. Bauer

The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) and the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) are 2 of the most common psychological tests used in clinical care and research in neurology. Newly revised versions of both instruments (WAIS-IV and WMS-IV) have recently been published and are increasingly being adopted by the neuropsychology community. There have been significant changes in the structure and content of both scales, leading to the potential for inaccurate patient classification if algorithms developed using their predecessors are employed. There are presently insufficient clinical data in neurologic populations to insure their appropriate application to neuropsychological evaluations. We provide a perspective on these important new neuropsychological instruments, comment on the pressures to adopt these tests in the absence of an appropriate evidence base supporting their incremental validity, and describe the potential negative impact on both patient care and continuing research applications.


Archive | 1986

The Cognitive Psychophysiology of Prosopagnosia

Russell M. Bauer

Prosopagnosia is a rare neurobehavioral syndrome in which a patient with brain damage becomes unable to recognize previously familiar persons by visual reference to their facial features (Bodamer, 1947; Bauer & Rubens, 1985). It is distinct from disorders in the perceptual processing of previously unfamiliar faces (Benton, 1980) and is dissociable from specific impairments in learning new faces seen as part of a more general visual recent memory deficit (Ross, 1980). In many cases, the disorder extends to famous faces and may even prevent identification of the patient’s own face in a mirror. Prosopagnosics invariably recognize faces as faces, and are able to achieve immediate and certain recognition when they hear the person’s voice or when some informative extrafacial visual cue (clothing, gait, etc.) is available. Prosopagnosia cannot be solely attributed to aphasic misnaming or to perceptual impairment, since tests of language and visual perception are usually performed at normal or near-normal levels (cf. Bauer & Rubens, 1985 for review).


Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology | 2012

Computerized Neuropsychological Assessment Devices: Joint Position Paper of the American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology and the National Academy of Neuropsychology

Russell M. Bauer; Grant L. Iverson; Alison N. Cernich; Laurence M. Binder; Ronald M. Ruff; Richard I. Naugle

This joint position paper of the American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology and the National Academy of Neuropsychology sets forth our position on appropriate standards and conventions for computerized neuropsychological assessment devices (CNADs). In this paper, we first define CNADs and distinguish them from examiner-administered neuropsychological instruments. We then set forth position statements on eight key issues relevant to the development and use of CNADs in the healthcare setting. These statements address (a) device marketing and performance claims made by developers of CNADs; (b) issues involved in appropriate end-users for administration and interpretation of CNADs; (c) technical (hardware/software/firmware) issues; (d) privacy, data security, identity verification, and testing environment; (e) psychometric development issues, especially reliability, and validity; (f) cultural, experiential, and disability factors affecting examinee interaction with CNADs; (g) use of computerized testing and reporting services; and (h) the need for checks on response validity and effort in the CNAD environment. This paper is intended to provide guidance for test developers and users of CNADs that will promote accurate and appropriate use of computerized tests in a way that maximizes clinical utility and minimizes risks of misuse. The positions taken in this paper are put forth with an eye toward balancing the need to make validated CNADs accessible to otherwise underserved patients with the need to ensure that such tests are developed and utilized competently, appropriately, and with due concern for patient welfare and quality of care.

Collaboration


Dive into the Russell M. Bauer's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Richard W. Briggs

University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge