Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Sara Jo Nixon is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Sara Jo Nixon.


Drug and Alcohol Dependence | 1995

Neuropsychological performance of recently abstinent alcoholics and cocaine abusers

William W. Beatty; Vickie M. Katzung; Valerie J. Moreland; Sara Jo Nixon

To examine possible influences of premorbid and comorbid factors on the neuropsychological test performance of recently abstinent (3-5 weeks) drug abusers, we studied 24 alcoholics, 23 cocaine abusers, and 22 healthy controls of comparable age and education. Both alcoholics and cocaine abusers performed significantly more poorly than controls on most measures of learning and memory, problem solving and abstraction and perceptual-motor speed, but the groups did not differ on the measure of sustained attention. Correlational analyses revealed no significant relationships between measures of childhood and residual hyperactivity and neuropsychological performance; scores on the Beck Depression Inventory were related only to performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. The findings indicate that abuse of cocaine or alcohol is associated with deficits on neuropsychological tests which cannot be attributed to specific premorbid or comorbid factors such as depression or childhood or residual attention deficit disorder.


Psychiatry MMC | 2001

Television Exposure in Children after a Terrorist Incident

Betty Pfefferbaum; Sara Jo Nixon; Rick Tivis; Debby E. Doughty; Robert S. Pynoos; Robin H. Gurwitch; David W. Foy

Abstract This study examined the influence of bomb-related television viewing in the context of physical and emotional exposure on posttraumatic stress symptoms—intrusion, avoidance, and arousal—in middle school students following the 1995 Oklahoma City bombing. Over 2,000 middle school students in Oklahoma City were surveyed 7 weeks after the incident. The primary outcome measures were the total posttraumatic stress symptom score and symptom cluster scores at the time of assessment. Bomb-related television viewing in the aftermath of the disaster was extensive. Both emotional and television exposure were associated with post-traumatic stress at 7 weeks. Among children with no physical or emotional exposure, the degree of television exposure was directly related to posttraumatic stress symptomatology. These findings suggest that television viewing in the aftermath of a disaster may make a small contribution to subsequent posttraumatic stress symptomatology in children or that increased television viewing may be a sign of current distress and that it should be monitored. Future research should examine further whether early symptoms predict increased television viewing and/or whether television viewing predicts subsequent symptoms.


International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health | 2010

Chronic Cigarette Smoking: Implications for Neurocognition and Brain Neurobiology

Timothy C. Durazzo; Dieter J. Meyerhoff; Sara Jo Nixon

Compared to the substantial volume of research on the general health consequences associated with chronic smoking, little research has been specifically devoted to the investigation of its effects on human neurobiology and neurocognition. This review summarizes the peer-reviewed literature on the neurocognitive and neurobiological implications of chronic cigarette smoking in cohorts that were not seeking treatment for substance use or psychiatric disorders. Studies that specifically assessed the neurocognitive or neurobiological (with emphasis on computed tomography and magnetic resonance-based neuroimaging studies) consequences of chronic smoking are highlighted. Chronic cigarette smoking appears to be associated with deficiencies in executive functions, cognitive flexibility, general intellectual abilities, learning and/or memory processing speed, and working memory. Chronic smoking is related to global brain atrophy and to structural and biochemical abnormalities in anterior frontal regions, subcortical nuclei and commissural white matter. Chronic smoking may also be associated with an increased risk for various forms of neurodegenerative diseases. The existing literature is limited by inconsistent accounting for potentially confounding biomedical and psychiatric conditions, focus on cross-sectional studies with middle aged and older adults and the absence of studies concurrently assessing neurocognitive, neurobiological and genetic factors in the same cohort. Consequently, the mechanisms promoting the neurocognitive and neurobiological abnormalities reported in chronic smokers are unclear. Longitudinal studies are needed to determine if the smoking-related neurobiological and neurocognitive abnormalities increase over time and/or show recovery with sustained smoking cessation.


Journal of Behavioral Health Services & Research | 2000

Predictors of post-traumatic stress symptoms in Oklahoma City: exposure, social support, peri-traumatic responses.

Phebe Tucker; Betty Pfefferbaum; Sara Jo Nixon; Warren Dickson

Eighty-five adults seeking mental health assistance six months after the Oklahoma City bombing were assessed to determine which of three groups of variables (exposure, peri-traumatic responses, and social support) predicted development of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms. Variables most highly associated with subsequent PTSD symptoms included having been injured (among exposure variables), feeling nervous or afraid (peri-traumatic responses), and responding that counseling helped (support variables). Combining primary predictors in the three areas, PTSD symptoms were more likely to occur in those reporting counseling to help and those feeling nervous or afraid at the time of the bombing. Implications of these findings are discussed for behavioral health administrators and clinicians planning service delivery to groups of victims seeking mental health intervention after terrorist attacks and other disasters.


Journal of Urban Health-bulletin of The New York Academy of Medicine | 2002

Exposure and peritraumatic response as predictors of posttraumatic stress in children following the 1995 Oklahoma City bombing

Betty Pfefferbaum; Debby E. Doughty; Chandrashekar Reddy; Nilam Patel; Robin H. Gurwitch; Sara Jo Nixon; Rick Tivis

Studies have demonstrated a positive relationship between exposure and posttraumatic stress, but ones subjective appraisal of danger and threat at the time of exposure may be a better predictor of posttraumatic stress than more objective measures of exposure. We examined the role of peritraumatic response in posttraumatic stress reactions in over 2,000 middle school children 7 weeks after the 1995 Oklahoma City, Oklahoma, bombing. While many children reported hearing and feeling the blast and knowing direct victims, most were in school at the time of the explosion and therefore were not in direct physical proximity to the incident. Physical, interpersonal, and television exposure accounted for 12% of the total variance in our measure of posttraumatic stress when peritraumatic response was ignored. Peritraumatic response and television exposure accounted for 25% of the total variance, and physical and interpersonal exposure were not significant in this context. These findings suggest the importance of peritraumatic response in childrens reactions to terrorism. These carly responses can be used to help determine which children may experience difficulty over time.


Neurocase | 2003

Musical Skill in Dementia: A Violinist Presumed to Have Alzheimer’s Disease Learns to Play a New Song

Anne Cowles; William W. Beatty; Sara Jo Nixon; Lanna J. Lutz; Jason Paulk; Kayla Paulk; Elliott D. Ross

Previous studies have described patients with possible or probable Alzheimer’s disease (AD) who continued to play familiar songs skillfully, despite their dementias. There are no reports about patients with dementia who successfully learned to play new songs, and two papers describe failures of patients with AD to learn to play a new song although they continued to play familiar songs competently. In the present paper we describe a moderately demented patient (SL) with probable AD who learned to play a song (Cossackaya!) on the violin that was published after the apparent onset of his dementia. He showed modest retention of the song at delays of 0 and 10 minutes. This contrasts with his profound disturbance in both recall and recognition on other anterograde memory tests (word lists, stories, figures, environmental sounds, sounds of musical instruments), and marked impairment on measures of remote memory (famous faces, autobiographical memory). SL showed milder deficits in confrontation naming, verbal fluency and attention, but no dyspraxia or aphasic comprehension deficits. Except for the Block Design test, his visuospatial skills were intact. SL’s learning of the new song in the absence of any evidence of episodic memory is reminiscent of patients with temporal lobe amnesia who show better memory for song melody than for lyrics or verse, although his retention was not as good.


Drug and Alcohol Dependence | 2012

A comprehensive assessment of neurocognition in middle-aged chronic cigarette smokers.

Timothy C. Durazzo; Dieter J. Meyerhoff; Sara Jo Nixon

BACKGROUND The majority of studies investigating the neurocognitive consequences of chronic smoking have been conducted with adults 60 years and older. Therefore, the scope of neurocognitive dysfunction associated with chronic cigarette smoking in middle age (i.e., 30-60 age range) has not been fully delineated. METHODS Twenty-seven (44±9 years of age; 4 females) non-smoking and 30 smoking (49±8 years of age; 4 females) participants completed a comprehensive neurocognitive battery and measures of fine motor dexterity and postural stability. All participants were free of biomedical or psychiatric conditions that may have influenced neurocognitive and motor function. RESULTS Smokers performed significantly worse than non-smokers on the following domains: auditory-verbal and visuospatial learning, visuospatial memory, cognitive efficiency, executive skills, general intelligence, processing speed, fine motor dexterity and postural stability. The differences between smokers and non-smokers evidenced moderate to strong effect sizes and were not mediated by age, education, vocational level, estimated verbal intelligence or alcohol consumption. In smokers, a greater number of lifetime years of smoking was related to poorer performance on measures of cognitive efficiency, processing speed and visuospatial skills. CONCLUSIONS Results from this middle-aged cohort replicated previous research and provides novel findings indicating that chronic smoking was associated with inferior performance on measures of general intelligence, visuospatial learning and memory and fine motor dexterity. Research that relates measures of neurobiological function/integrity to neurocognition is needed to better understand the mechanisms contributing to the poorer performance across multiple domains demonstrated by smokers.


Psychiatry Research-neuroimaging | 1996

Neurocognitive function in alcoholic, schizophrenic, and dually diagnosed patients

Sara Jo Nixon; H.Gene Hallford; Rick Tivis

Neurocognitive functions were compared in alcoholic, schizophrenic, and dually diagnosed (schizophrenic and alcoholic) patients and community control volunteers. Participants (N = 52, n = 13 in each group) completed a brief battery including two measures of neuropsychological function and two affective measures. The neuropsychological tests included the Trail-Making Test Forms A and B and a face-recognition test. The affective measures included assessment of anxiety and depression. The major objective was to compare the patterns of neurocognitive performance among the four groups. The groups did not differ in mean age (35.37) or years of education (12.46). Compared with the community control and alcoholic groups, the schizophrenic groups reported higher mean depression (12.92 vs. 5.54) and mean state anxiety scores (60.73 vs. 46.04). Control subjects were generally, although not always significantly, superior to the other groups. Contrary to expectations, alcoholic patients were not consistently better than the schizophrenic groups, and dually diagnosed participants were not more impaired than schizophrenic patients without substance abuse histories. Thus, the interaction of schizophrenia and substance abuse did not have an additive effect on neurocognitive performance assessed in this study. More research using this complete four-group design and a larger neurocognitive battery is needed.


Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research | 2005

Health Risks of Chronic Moderate and Heavy Alcohol Consumption: How Much Is Too Much?

Dieter J. Meyerhoff; Christiane Bode; Sara Jo Nixon; Eveline A. de Bruin; J. Christian Bode; Helmut K. Seitz

This article presents the proceedings of a symposium held at the meeting of the International Society for Biomedical Research on Alcoholism (ISBRA) in Mannheim, Germany, in October 2004. Most of what we know about the deleterious effects of alcohol in vivo has been gleaned from studies in sober alcoholics recruited from substance abuse treatment programs. Little is known about effects of chronic drinking in the moderate or heavy range encountered in a much larger fraction of modern society. Extrapolation of information on the adverse effects of chronic drinking on organ function from clinical samples to social drinkers in the general population has to be met with great skepticism, as it may lead to wrong conclusions about the chronic effects of alcohol in social drinkers. Several recent studies suggest that moderate alcohol consumption has certain beneficial health effects, whereas heavy social alcohol consumption has recently been associated with organ abnormalities and cognitive deficits. These social drinking effects have attracted great public interest; reports of benefits of moderate drinking have also inspired inappropriate publications by the media, including misleading advertisements by the alcohol producing and distributing industry. Although adverse effects of moderate to heavy drinking on heart, liver, and cancer development have attracted attention by clinicians and researchers for some time, its compromising effects on brain and cognition have only recently been studied. This symposium brought together researchers from different disciplines, who reviewed and presented new data on consequences of social drinking in the areas of clinical neuropsychology and behavior (Drs. Nixon and Meyerhoff), neurophysiology (Dr. Nixon, Ms. De Bruin), neuroimaging (Ms. de Bruin, Dr. Meyerhoff), hepatic disease (Dr. Bode), and cancer (Dr. Seitz). The symposium aimed to clarify both the potential health benefits of moderate alcohol consumption and risks of moderate and heavy drinking on proper organ function and to provide insights and new data to practicing physicians and public health authorities for education on problem drinking.


Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research | 2003

Cognitive Efficiency in Stimulant Abusers With and Without Alcohol Dependence

Andrea Lawton-Craddock; Sara Jo Nixon; Rick Tivis

BACKGROUND Although previous studies have found stimulant (i.e., cocaine, methamphetamine) abusers and alcoholics to have neuropsychological deficits, research examining which cognitive abilities are most affected by concurrent exposure to these substances is lacking. To address this issue, detoxified men and women who met criteria for dependence of (a) alcohol only (ALC) (n = 15); (b) stimulants only (STIM) (n = 15); and (c) both alcohol and stimulants (A/STIM) (n = 15) were compared with age- and education-matched community controls (n = 15). METHODS Tasks that measured visual spatial skills, problem-solving and abstraction, short-term memory, cognitive flexibility, and gross motor speed were administered to participants. For each test, both speed and accuracy were assessed and an efficiency ratio (accuracy/time) was derived. Based on an average of these efficiency ratios, an overall performance index of cognitive efficiency was obtained. RESULTS Overall, controls performed more efficiently than all other groups. However, they were statistically significantly better only in relation to the A/STIM and STIM groups (p < 0.01). Individual comparisons revealed that the ALC group performed significantly better than the STIM group, although the ALC group did not differ from either the control or A/STIM groups (p </= 0.05). This pattern of results was relatively consistent across the individual subtests of problem-solving/abstraction, short-term memory, and cognitive flexibility. CONCLUSIONS As expected, substance abuse was associated with cognitive inefficiency. More importantly, these findings suggest that the cognitive effects of chronic stimulant abuse are not additive with those of alcohol abuse. That is, singly addicted stimulant abusers demonstrated similar or greater neurocognitive impairments than individuals who abuse alcohol and stimulants concurrently. The reason for this pattern is speculative but may be attributed to alcohols opposing actions on cerebrovascular effects brought on by stimulant abuse.

Collaboration


Dive into the Sara Jo Nixon's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Oscar A. Parsons

University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Rick Tivis

University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ben Lewis

University of Florida

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Betty Pfefferbaum

Washington University in St. Louis

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

William W. Beatty

North Dakota State University

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge