Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Sarah E. Berger is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Sarah E. Berger.


Developmental Psychology | 2003

Infants use handrails as tools in a locomotor task.

Sarah E. Berger; Karen E. Adolph

In 2 experiments the authors demonstrated that adaptive locomotion can involve means-ends problem solving. Sixteen-month-old toddlers crossed bridges of varying widths in the presence or absence of a handrail. Babies attempted wider bridges more often than narrow ones, and attempts on narrow bridges depended on handrail presence. Toddlers had longer latencies, examined the bridge and handrail more closely, and modified their gait when bridges were narrow and/or the handrail was unavailable. Infants who explored the bridge and handrail before stepping onto the bridge and devised alternative bridge-crossing strategies were more likely to cross successfully. Results challenge traditional conceptualizations of tools: Babies used the handrail as a means for augmenting balance and for carrying out an otherwise impossible goal-directed task.


Child Development | 2010

Bridging the Gap: Solving Spatial Means-Ends Relations in a Locomotor Task

Sarah E. Berger; Karen E. Adolph; Alisan E. Kavookjian

Using a means-means-ends problem-solving task, this study examined whether 16-month-old walking infants (N = 28) took into account the width of a bridge as a means for crossing a precipice and the location of a handrail as a means for augmenting balance on a narrow bridge. Infants were encouraged to cross from one platform to another over narrow and wide bridges located at various distances from a wooden handrail. Infants attempted to walk over the wide bridge more often than the narrow one and when the handrail was within reach. Infants demonstrated parallel problem solving by modifying exploratory behaviors and bridge-crossing strategies that simultaneously accounted for the spatial and functional relations between body and bridge, body and handrail, and bridge and handrail.


Pediatric Physical Therapy | 2015

Sitting Postural Control Affects the Development of Focused Attention in Children With Cerebral Palsy

Swati M. Surkar; Christina Edelbrock; Nicholas Stergiou; Sarah E. Berger; Regina T. Harbourne

Purpose: To investigate whether focused attention (FA) changes over time as sitting postural control improves and whether an impairment in sitting postural control affects the development of FA in children with cerebral palsy (CP). Methods: Nineteen children with CP, mean ages 21.47 months, were assessed for FA and sitting scores pre- and postintervention. Results: Longest, total, and global FA increased and frequency of FA decreased in children who achieved independent sitting. However, children who achieved mobility postintervention exhibited a decrease in longest FA and an increase in frequency of FA. Conclusion: Sitting postural control and the development of FA appear associated in children with CP. The increase in FA may signal a key opportunity for learning and attending to objects. However, the time of early mobility may interrupt these long periods of attention, resulting in less sustained attention to objects.


Archive | 2018

Cognition–Action Trade-Offs Reflect Organization of Attention in Infancy

Sarah E. Berger; Regina T. Harbourne; Melissa N. Horger

This chapter discusses what cognition-action trade-offs in infancy reveal about the organization and developmental trajectory of attention. We focus on internal attention because this aspect is most relevant to the immediate concerns of infancy, such as fluctuating levels of expertise, balancing multiple taxing skills simultaneously, learning how to control attention under variable conditions, and coordinating distinct psychological domains. Cognition-action trade-offs observed across the life span include perseveration during skill emergence, errors and inefficient strategies during decision making, and the allocation of resources when attention is taxed. An embodied cognitive-load account interprets these behavioral patterns as a result of limited attentional resources allocated across simultaneous, taxing task demands. For populations where motor errors could be costly, like infants and the elderly, attention is typically devoted to motor demands with errors occurring in the cognitive domain. In contrast, healthy young adults tend to preserve their cognitive performance by modifying their actions.


Archive | 2005

Physical and motor development

Karen E. Adolph; Sarah E. Berger


Infancy | 2004

Demands on Finite Cognitive Capacity Cause Infants' Perseverative Errors

Sarah E. Berger


Progress in Brain Research | 2007

Learning and development in infant locomotion

Sarah E. Berger; Karen E. Adolph


Developmental Psychology | 2010

Locomotor Expertise Predicts Infants' Perseverative Errors.

Sarah E. Berger


Child Development | 2005

Out of the Toolbox: Toddlers Differentiate Wobbly and Wooden Handrails

Sarah E. Berger; Karen E. Adolph; Sharon A. Lobo


Developmental Science | 2011

Developmental Continuity? Crawling, Cruising, and Walking

Karen E. Adolph; Sarah E. Berger; Andrew J. Leo

Collaboration


Dive into the Sarah E. Berger's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Brian Chin

Stony Brook University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jennifer Ducz

City University of New York

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge