Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Sarah K. VanSchyndel is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Sarah K. VanSchyndel.


Child Development | 2016

Prosocial Motivation: Inferences From an Opaque Body of Work

Nancy Eisenberg; Sarah K. VanSchyndel; Tracy L. Spinrad

Because motivations for prosocial actions typically are unclear, sometimes even to actors but especially for observers, it is difficult to study prosocial motivation. This article reviews research that provides evidence regarding childrens motives for prosocial behaviors. First, we present a heuristic model to classify motives on the dimension of reflecting altruistic (with the ultimate goal of benefiting another) to egoism (the ultimate goal of benefiting the self) goals; in addition, we briefly discuss classifying motives based on a continuum of morality. Next, we review findings indicating the existence of a number of different motives in our model and briefly discuss developmental issues, when possible. Future directions for the study of prosocial motivation are proposed.


Journal of Research in Personality | 2017

Relations of positive and negative expressivity and effortful control to kindergarteners’ student–teacher relationship, academic engagement, and externalizing problems at school

Anjolii Diaz; Nancy Eisenberg; Carlos Valiente; Sarah K. VanSchyndel; Tracy L. Spinrad; Rebecca H. Berger; Maciel M. Hernández; Kassondra M. Silva; Jody Southworth

The current study examined the role of naturally-occurring negative and positive emotion expressivity in kindergarten and childrens effortful control (EC) on their relationships with teachers, academic engagement, and problems behaviors in school. Further, the potential moderating role of EC on these important school outcomes was assessed. Emotion and engagement were observed at school. EC was assessed by multiple methods. Teachers reported on their student-teacher relationships and students externalizing behaviors. Childrens emotion expressivity and EC were related to engagement and relationships with teachers as well as behavioral problems at school. Children low in EC may be particularly vulnerable to the poor outcomes associated with relatively intense emotion expressivity as they struggle to manage their emotions and behaviors in the classroom.


Emotion | 2014

Children’s Negative Emotions and Ego-Resiliency: Longitudinal Relations With Social Competence

Zoe E. Taylor; Nancy Eisenberg; Sarah K. VanSchyndel; Natalie D. Eggum-Wilkens; Tracy L. Spinrad

We examined the relations of negative emotions in toddlerhood to the development of ego-resiliency and social competence across early childhood. Specifically, we addressed whether fear and anger/frustration in 30-month-old children (N = 213) was associated with the development of ego-resiliency across 4 time points (42 to 84 months), and, in turn, whether ego-resiliency predicted social competence at 84 months. Child anger/frustration negatively predicted the intercept of ego-resiliency at 42 months (controlling for prior ego-resiliency at 18 months) as well as the slope. Fear did not significantly predict either the intercept or slope of ego-resiliency in the structural model, although it was positively correlated with anger/frustration and was negatively related to ego-resiliency in zero-order correlations. The slope of ego-resiliency was positively related to childrens social competence at 84 months; however, the intercept of ego-resiliency (set at 42 months) was not a significant predictor of later social competence. Furthermore, the slope of ego-resiliency mediated the relations between anger/frustration and childrens later social competence. The results suggest that individual differences in anger/frustration might contribute to the development of ego-resiliency, which, in turn, is associated with childrens social competence.


International Journal of Behavioral Development | 2017

Concurrent and longitudinal associations of peers’ acceptance with emotion and effortful control in kindergarten

Maciel M. Hernández; Nancy Eisenberg; Carlos Valiente; Anjolii Diaz; Sarah K. VanSchyndel; Rebecca H. Berger; Nathan Terrell; Kassondra M. Silva; Tracy L. Spinrad; Jody Southworth

The purpose of the study was to evaluate bidirectional associations between peer acceptance and both emotion and effortful control during kindergarten (N = 301). In both the fall and spring semesters, we obtained peer nominations of acceptance, measures of positive and negative emotion based on naturalistic observations in school (i.e., classroom, lunch/recess), and observers’ reports of effortful control (i.e., inhibitory control, attention focusing) and emotions (i.e., positive, negative). In structural equation panel models, peer acceptance in fall predicted higher effortful control in spring. Effortful control in fall did not predict peer acceptance in spring. Negative emotion predicted lower peer acceptance across time for girls but not for boys. Peer acceptance did not predict negative or positive emotion over time. In addition, we tested interactions between positive or negative emotion and effortful control predicting peer acceptance. Positive emotion predicted higher peer acceptance for children low in effortful control.


Social Development | 2017

Observed Emotions as Predictors of Quality of Kindergartners’ Social Relationships

Maciel M. Hernández; Nancy Eisenberg; Carlos Valiente; Tracy L. Spinrad; Sarah K. VanSchyndel; Anjolii Diaz; Kassondra M. Silva; Rebecca H. Berger; Jody Southworth

This study evaluated whether positive and anger emotional frequency (the proportion of instances an emotion was observed) and intensity (the strength of an emotion when it was observed) uniquely predicted social relationships among kindergarteners (N = 301). Emotions were observed as naturally occurring at school in the fall term and multiple reporters (peers and teachers) provided information on quality of relationships with children in the spring term. In structural equation models, positive emotion frequency, but not positive emotion intensity, was positively related to peer acceptance and negatively related to peer rejection. In contrast, the frequency of anger provided unique positive prediction of teacher-student conflict and negative prediction of peer acceptance. Furthermore, anger intensity negatively predicted teacher-student closeness and positively predicted teacher-student conflict. Implications for promoting social relationships in school are discussed.


International Journal of Behavioral Development | 2017

Children’s sleep and academic achievement: The moderating role of effortful control

Anjolii Diaz; Rebecca H. Berger; Carlos Valiente; Nancy Eisenberg; Sarah K. VanSchyndel; Chun Tao; Tracy L. Spinrad; Leah D. Doane; Marilyn S. Thompson; Kassondra M. Silva; Jody Southworth

Poor sleep is thought to interfere with children’s learning and academic achievement (AA). However, existing research and theory indicate there are factors that may mitigate the academic risk associated with poor sleep. The purpose of this study was to examine the moderating role of children’s effortful control (EC) on the relation between sleep and AA in young children. One hundred and three 4.5- to 7-year-olds (M = 5.98 years, SD = 0.61) wore a wrist-based actigraph for five continuous weekday nights. Teachers and coders reported on children’s EC. EC was also assessed with a computer-based task at school. Additionally, we obtained a standardized measure of children’s AA. There was a positive main effect of sleep efficiency to AA. Several relations between sleep and AA were moderated by EC and examination of the simple slopes indicated that the negative relation between sleep and AA was only significant at low levels of EC.


Early Childhood Research Quarterly | 2017

Elementary students’ effortful control and academic achievement: The mediating role of teacher–student relationship quality

Maciel M. Hernández; Carlos Valiente; Nancy Eisenberg; Rebecca H. Berger; Tracy L. Spinrad; Sarah K. VanSchyndel; Kassondra M. Silva; Jody Southworth; Marilyn S. Thompson

This study evaluated the association between effortful control in kindergarten and academic achievement one year later (N = 301), and whether teacher-student closeness and conflict in kindergarten mediated the association. Parents, teachers, and observers reported on childrens effortful control, and teachers reported on their perceived levels of closeness and conflict with students. Students completed the passage comprehension and applied problems subtests of the Woodcock-Johnson tests of achievement, as well as a behavioral measure of effortful control. Analytical models predicting academic achievement were estimated using a structural equation model framework. Effortful control positively predicted academic achievement even when controlling for prior achievement and other covariates. Mediation hypotheses were tested in a separate model; effortful control positively predicted teacher-student closeness and strongly, negatively predicted teacher-student conflict. Teacher-student closeness and effortful control, but not teacher-student conflict, had small, positive associations with academic achievement. Effortful control also indirectly predicted higher academic achievement through its positive effect on teacher-student closeness and via its positive relation to early academic achievement. The findings suggest that teacher-student closeness is one mechanism by which effortful control is associated with academic achievement. Effortful control was also a consistent predictor of academic achievement, beyond prior achievement levels and controlling for teacher-student closeness and conflict, with implications for intervention programs on fostering regulation and achievement concurrently.


Early Education and Development | 2018

Sleep Duration Moderates the Association Between Children’s Temperament and Academic Achievement

Rebecca H. Berger; Anjolii Diaz; Carlos Valiente; Nancy Eisenberg; Tracy L. Spinrad; Marilyn S. Thompson; Maciel M. Hernández; Sarah K. VanSchyndel; Jody Southworth

ABSTRACT Research Findings: The primary goal of this study was to determine whether sleep duration moderates the relations of 2 dimensions of children’s temperament—shyness and negative emotion—to academic achievement. In the autumn, parents and teachers reported on kindergartners’ and 1st graders’ (N = 103) shyness and negative emotion and research assistants observed negative emotion in the classroom. In the spring, children wore actigraphs that measured their sleep for 5 consecutive school nights, and they completed the Woodcock–Johnson III Tests of Achievement. Interactions between temperament and sleep duration predicting academic achievement were computed. Interactions of sleep duration with parent-reported shyness, teacher-reported negative emotion, and observed negative emotion indicated that the negative relations of shyness or negative emotion to academic achievement were strongest when children slept less. Practice or Policy: Results suggest that sleep duration may be an important bioregulatory factor to consider in young children’s early academic achievement.


Early Education and Development | 2018

Balance in Positive Emotional Expressivity Across School Contexts Relates to Kindergartners’ Adjustment

Maciel M. Hernández; Nancy Eisenberg; Carlos Valiente; Tracy L. Spinrad; Rebecca H. Berger; Sarah K. VanSchyndel; Marilyn S. Thompson; Jody Southworth; Kassondra M. Silva

ABSTRACT Positive emotional expressivity has been associated with increased social competence and decreased maladjustment in childhood. However, a few researchers have found null or even positive associations between positive emotional expressivity and maladjustment, which suggests that there may be nuanced associations of positive expressivity, perhaps as a function of the social context in which it is expressed. We examined whether observed positive emotional expressivity balance across peer-oriented/recreational and learning contexts predicted kindergarten children’s adjustment (N = 301). Research Findings: Higher positive expressivity during lunch/recess compared to positive expressivity in the classroom was associated with lower teacher–student conflict, externalizing behaviors, and depressive symptoms. In addition, overall positive emotional expressivity predicted lower externalizing behaviors as well as lower depressive and anxiety symptoms. Practice or Policy: The results suggest the importance of assessing observed positive emotional expressivity in context as a potential indicator of children’s maladjustment risk and the need for children to adapt their emotions to different contexts. Implications for assessing and supporting positive emotional expression balance and training emotional regulation in school are discussed.


Journal of Educational Psychology | 2017

Trajectories of the Expression of Negative Emotion From Kindergarten to First Grade: Associations With Academic Outcomes

Maciel M. Hernández; Nancy Eisenberg; Carlos Valiente; Marilyn S. Thompson; Tracy L. Spinrad; Kevin J. Grimm; Sarah K. VanSchyndel; Rebecca H. Berger; Kassondra M. Silva; Armando Pina; Jody Southworth; Diana E. Gal

We examined individual trajectories, across four time points, of children’s (N = 301) expression of negative emotion in classroom settings and whether these trajectories predicted their observed school engagement, teacher-reported academic skills, and passage comprehension assessed with a standardized measure in first grade. In latent growth curve analyses, negative expressivity declined from kindergarten to first grade, with significant individual differences in trajectories. Negative expressivity in kindergarten inversely predicted first-grade school engagement and teacher-reported academic skills, and the slope of negative expressivity from kindergarten to first grade inversely predicted school engagement (e.g., increasing negative expressivity was associated with lower school engagement). In addition, we examined whether prior academic functioning in kindergarten moderated the association between negative expressivity (level in kindergarten and change over time) and academic functioning in first grade. The slope of negative expressivity was negatively associated with first-grade school engagement and passage comprehension for children who had lower kindergarten school engagement and passage comprehension, respectively, but was unrelated for those with higher academic functioning in kindergarten. That is, for children who had lower kindergarten school engagement and passage comprehension, greater declines in negative expressivity were associated with higher first-grade school engagement and passage comprehension, respectively. The findings suggest that negative emotional expressivity in school is associated with academic outcomes in first grade, and, in some cases, this association is more pronounced for children who had lower kindergarten academic functioning.

Collaboration


Dive into the Sarah K. VanSchyndel's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Armando Pina

Arizona State University

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge