Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Sarah M. Zala is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Sarah M. Zala.


Ecology Letters | 2015

Brain size affects female but not male survival under predation threat

Alexander Kotrschal; Severine D. Buechel; Sarah M. Zala; Alberto Corral-López; Dustin J. Penn; Niclas Kolm

There is remarkable diversity in brain size among vertebrates, but surprisingly little is known about how ecological species interactions impact the evolution of brain size. Using guppies, artificially selected for large and small brains, we determined how brain size affects survival under predation threat in a naturalistic environment. We cohoused mixed groups of small- and large-brained individuals in six semi-natural streams with their natural predator, the pike cichlid, and monitored survival in weekly censuses over 5 months. We found that large-brained females had 13.5% higher survival compared to small-brained females, whereas the brain size had no discernible effect on male survival. We suggest that large-brained females have a cognitive advantage that allows them to better evade predation, whereas large-brained males are more colourful, which may counteract any potential benefits of brain size. Our study provides the first experimental evidence that trophic interactions can affect the evolution of brain size.


Behaviour | 2010

Reproductive behaviour of wild zebrafish (Danio rerio) in large tanks

Sophie Hutter; Dustin J. Penn; S. Magee; Sarah M. Zala

Summary Zebrafish are an intensively studied model organism, and yet there are no studies describing the reproductive behaviour of wild zebrafish. Here we investigated the reproductive behaviour of wild-caught zebrafish (Danio rerio) from India in large (1100 l) aquaria containing gravel and plants and low population densities. We observed the behaviour of focal individuals in groups of eight fish (equal sex ratio; 28 replicates; 42 h total observations), and found similarities to what has been previously described for domesticated fish in small aquaria. However, we also found several notable differences and, in particular, we found that the fish usually spawned in pairs rather than in groups, and we found evidence for sexual selection. We conducted additional observations of wild-derived fish in smaller, conventional aquaria (17 l) and high densities, and found that group spawning is more common in these conditions. Taken together, our observations suggest that high rates of group spawning may be an artefact of small aquaria, which may reduce or eliminate sexual selection by giving more opportunities for sneaky males and preventing females’ from choosing their mates.


Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology | 2008

Exposing males to female scent increases the cost of controlling Salmonella infection in wild house mice

Sarah M. Zala; Wayne K. Potts; Dustin J. Penn

Secondary sexual characters often provide indicators of a male’s resistance to infectious diseases to rivals and potential mates, but it is unclear why. It is often suggested that males honestly signal their health due to energetic and other physiological trade-offs between investing into secondary sexual traits vs resistance to infectious diseases. Our aim was to determine whether such a trade-off exists using wild-derived male house mice (Mus domesticus). We exposed male mice to female scent, a manipulation that induces elevations in testosterone concentration and the expression of a variety of testosterone-mediated secondary sexual traits, and tested whether this sexual stimulation impaired the males’ ability to resolve or cope with an experimental infection (Salmonella enterica). We kept the males on a controlled diet to prevent them from compensating by eating more food. We found that sexually stimulated males were able to control bacterial growth as effectively as sham-stimulated controls; however, to do so, they lost more body mass during infection compared to the controls. In contrast, we found no evidence that sexual stimulation reduced the body mass of uninfected male mice. These results indicate that males’ responses to female odor are not immunosuppressive per se, yet they increase the energetic costs of controlling infection. Our findings support the idea that there is a physiological trade-off between secondary sexual signaling vs resistance to infectious diseases and suggest that studies using only immunocompetence assays might fail to detect such energetic trade-offs.


Journal of Ethology | 2011

Sex recognition in zebrafish (Danio rerio)

Sophie Hutter; Sarah M. Zala; Dustin J. Penn

We investigated sex recognition in female zebrafish (Danio rerio) to better understand the underlying sensory mechanisms and identify male secondary sexual traits. Females were simultaneously presented with two fish, a male and a female, in a flow-chamber apparatus, and females’ relative attraction towards males was observed under different conditions. With domesticated fish, females were more attracted to males when presented with both visual and chemosensory cues from stimulus fish. They still discriminated the sexes when only visual cues were provided, but not when white ambient light was changed to yellow, indicating that colour plays a role. Sex discrimination under yellow light was improved when chemosensory cues were also provided. Surprisingly, females’ attraction to males was not more pronounced in the morning when mating occurs. Domesticated females discriminated the sexes when presented with wild-derived, as well as domesticated fish, whereas wild-derived females did not show any biases for domesticated or wild-derived males. Behavioural observations indicated that the wild-derived females were distressed, which explains their lack of attraction to males. In summary, domesticated female zebrafish discriminated the sexes using both visual (body colour) and olfactory cues; however, wild-derived zebrafish were too distressed for behavioural experiments under these laboratory conditions.


PLOS ONE | 2017

Sex-dependent modulation of ultrasonic vocalizations in house mice (Mus musculus musculus)

Sarah M. Zala; Doris Reitschmidt; Anton Noll; Peter Balazs; Dustin J. Penn

House mice (Mus musculus) emit ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs), which are surprisingly complex and have features of bird song, but their functions are not well understood. Previous studies have reported mixed evidence on whether there are sex differences in USV emission, though vocalization rate or other features may depend upon whether potential receivers are of the same or opposite sex. We recorded the USVs of wild-derived adult house mice (F1 of wild-caught Mus musculus musculus), and we compared the vocalizations of males and females in response to a stimulus mouse of the same- or opposite-sex. To detect and quantify vocalizations, we used an algorithm that automatically detects USVs (Automatic Mouse Ultrasound Detector or A-MUD). We found high individual variation in USV emission rates (4 to 2083 elements/10 min trial) and a skewed distribution, with most mice (60%) emitting few (≤50) elements. We found no differences in the rates of calling between the sexes overall, but mice of both sexes emitted vocalizations at a higher rate and higher frequencies during opposite- compared to same-sex interactions. We also observed a trend toward higher amplitudes by males when presented with a male compared to a female stimulus. Our results suggest that mice modulate the rate and frequency of vocalizations depending upon the sex of potential receivers.


PLOS ONE | 2017

Automatic mouse ultrasound detector (A-MUD): A new tool for processing rodent vocalizations

Sarah M. Zala; Doris Reitschmidt; Anton Noll; Peter Balazs; Dustin J. Penn

House mice (Mus musculus) emit complex ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) during social and sexual interactions, which have features similar to bird song (i.e., they are composed of several different types of syllables, uttered in succession over time to form a pattern of sequences). Manually processing complex vocalization data is time-consuming and potentially subjective, and therefore, we developed an algorithm that automatically detects mouse ultrasonic vocalizations (Automatic Mouse Ultrasound Detector or A-MUD). A-MUD is a script that runs on STx acoustic software (S_TOOLS-STx version 4.2.2), which is free for scientific use. This algorithm improved the efficiency of processing USV files, as it was 4–12 times faster than manual segmentation, depending upon the size of the file. We evaluated A-MUD error rates using manually segmented sound files as a ‘gold standard’ reference, and compared them to a commercially available program. A-MUD had lower error rates than the commercial software, as it detected significantly more correct positives, and fewer false positives and false negatives. The errors generated by A-MUD were mainly false negatives, rather than false positives. This study is the first to systematically compare error rates for automatic ultrasonic vocalization detection methods, and A-MUD and subsequent versions will be made available for the scientific community.


Animal Behaviour | 2004

Abnormal behaviours induced by chemical pollution: a review of the evidence and new challenges

Sarah M. Zala; Dustin J. Penn


Behavioral Ecology | 2004

Scent-marking displays provide honest signals of health and infection

Sarah M. Zala; Wayne K. Potts; Dustin J. Penn


Brain Behavior and Immunity | 2008

Genetic resistance to infection influences a male's sexual attractiveness and modulation of testosterone.

Sarah M. Zala; Benjamin K. Chan; Staci D. Bilbo; Wayne K. Potts; Randy J. Nelson; Dustin J. Penn


Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology | 2015

Female house mice initially shun infected males, but do not avoid mating with them

Sarah M. Zala; Amber Bilak; Michael E. Perkins; Wayne K. Potts; Dustin J. Penn

Collaboration


Dive into the Sarah M. Zala's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Dustin J. Penn

University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Sophie Hutter

Austrian Academy of Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Peter Balazs

Austrian Academy of Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

S. Magee

Austrian Academy of Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Attila Hettyey

Eötvös Loránd University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Randy J. Nelson

The Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge