Scott F. Perkins
Vanderbilt University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Scott F. Perkins.
NeuroImage | 2017
Linh C. Dang; Jaime J. Castrellon; Scott F. Perkins; Nam T. Le; Ronald L. Cowan; David H. Zald; Gregory R. Samanez-Larkin
Abstract Physical activity has been shown to ameliorate dopaminergic degeneration in non‐human animal models. However, the effects of regular physical activity on normal age‐related changes in dopamine function in humans are unknown. Here we present cross‐sectional data from forty‐four healthy human subjects between 23 and 80 years old, showing that typical age‐related dopamine D2 receptor loss, assessed with PET [18 F]fallypride, was significantly reduced in physically active adults compared to less active adults.
International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research | 2018
Benjamin B. Lahey; David H. Zald; Scott F. Perkins; Victoria Villalta-Gil; Katherine B. Werts; Carol A. Van Hulle; Paul J. Rathouz; Brooks Applegate; Quetzal A. Class; Holly E. Poore; Ashley L. Watts; Irwin D. Waldman
There is evidence that models of psychopathology specifying a general factor and specific second‐order factors fit better than competing structural models. Nonetheless, additional tests are needed to examine the generality and boundaries of the general factor model. In a selected second wave of a cohort study, first‐order dimensions of psychopathology symptoms in 499 23‐ to 31‐year‐old twins were analyzed. Using confirmatory factor analysis, a bifactor model specifying a general factor and specific internalizing and externalizing factors fit better than competing models. Factor loadings in this model were sex invariant despite greater variances in the specific internalizing factor among females and greater variances in the general and specific externalizing factors among males. The bifactor structure was robust to the exclusion of any single first‐order dimension of psychopathology. Furthermore, the results were essentially unchanged when all overlapping symptoms that define multiple disorders were excluded from symptom dimensions. Furthermore, the best‐fitting bifactor model also emerged in exploratory structural equation modeling with freely estimated cross‐loadings. The general factor of psychopathology was robust across variations in measurement and analysis.
eNeuro | 2017
Linh C. Dang; Gregory R. Samanez-Larkin; Jaime J. Castrellon; Scott F. Perkins; Ronald L. Cowan; Paul A. Newhouse; David H. Zald
Abstract Spontaneous eye blink rate (EBR) has been proposed as a noninvasive, inexpensive marker of dopamine functioning. Support for a relation between EBR and dopamine function comes from observations that EBR is altered in populations with dopamine dysfunction and EBR changes under a dopaminergic manipulation. However, the evidence across the literature is inconsistent and incomplete. A direct correlation between EBR and dopamine function has so far been observed only in nonhuman animals. Given significant interest in using EBR as a proxy for dopamine function, this study aimed to verify a direct association in healthy, human adults. Here we measured EBR in healthy human subjects whose dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2) availability was assessed with positron emission tomography (PET)-[18F]fallypride to examine the predictive power of EBR for DRD2 availability. Effects of the dopamine agonist bromocriptine on EBR also were examined to determine the responsiveness of EBR to dopaminergic stimulation and, in light of the hypothesized inverted-U profile of dopamine effects, the role of DRD2 availability in EBR responsivity to bromocriptine. Results from 20 subjects (age 33.6 ± 7.6 years, 9F) showed no relation between EBR and DRD2 availability. EBR also was not responsive to dopaminergic stimulation by bromocriptine, and individual differences in DRD2 availability did not modulate EBR responsivity to bromocriptine. Given that EBR is hypothesized to be particularly sensitive to DRD2 function, these findings suggest caution in using EBR as a proxy for dopamine function in healthy humans.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience | 2018
Kendra L. Seaman; Nickolas Brooks; Teresa Karrer; Jaime J. Castrellon; Scott F. Perkins; Linh C. Dang; Ming Hsu; David H. Zald; Gregory R. Samanez-Larkin
Abstract Every day, humans make countless decisions that require the integration of information about potential benefits (i.e. rewards) with other decision features (i.e. effort required, probability of an outcome or time delays). Here, we examine the overlap and dissociation of behavioral preferences and neural representations of subjective value in the context of three different decision features (physical effort, probability and time delays) in a healthy adult life span sample. While undergoing functional neuroimaging, participants (N = 75) made incentive compatible choices between a smaller monetary reward with lower physical effort, higher probability, or a shorter time delay versus a larger monetary reward with higher physical effort, lower probability, or a longer time delay. Behavioral preferences were estimated from observed choices, and subjective values were computed using individual hyperbolic discount functions. We found that discount rates were uncorrelated across tasks. Despite this apparent behavioral dissociation between preferences, we found overlapping subjective value-related activity in the medial prefrontal cortex across all three tasks. We found no consistent evidence for age differences in either preferences or the neural representations of subjective value across adulthood. These results suggest that while the tolerance of decision features is behaviorally dissociable, subjective value signals share a common representation across adulthood.
Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience | 2018
Linh C. Dang; Gregory R. Samanez-Larkin; Jaime J. Castrellon; Scott F. Perkins; Ronald L. Cowan; David H. Zald
Reward valuation, which underlies all value-based decision-making, has been associated with dopamine function in many studies of nonhuman animals, but there is relatively less direct evidence for an association in humans. Here, we measured dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2) availability in vivo in humans to examine relations between individual differences in dopamine receptor availability and neural activity associated with a measure of reward valuation, expected value (i.e., the product of reward magnitude and the probability of obtaining the reward). Fourteen healthy adult subjects underwent PET with [18F]fallypride, a radiotracer with strong affinity for DRD2, and fMRI (on a separate day) while performing a reward valuation task. [18F]fallypride binding potential, reflecting DRD2 availability, in the midbrain correlated positively with neural activity associated with expected value, specifically in the left ventral striatum/caudate. The present results provide in vivo evidence from humans showing midbrain dopamine characteristics are associated with reward valuation.
Physiology & Behavior | 2017
Linh C. Dang; Gregory R. Samanez-Larkin; Christopher T. Smith; Jaime J. Castrellon; Scott F. Perkins; Ronald L. Cowan; Daniel O. Claassen; David H. Zald
The fat mass and obesity associated gene (FTO) was the first gene identified by genome-wide association studies to correlate with higher body mass index (BMI) and increased odds of obesity. FTO remains the locus with the largest and most replicated effect on body weight, but the mechanism whereby FTO affects body weight and the development of obesity is not fully understood. Here we tested whether FTO is associated with differences in food cravings and a key aspect of dopamine function that has been hypothesized to influence food reward mechanisms. Moreover, as food cravings and dopamine function are known to decline with age, we explored effects of age on relations between FTO and food cravings and dopamine function. Seven-eight healthy subjects between 22 and 83years old completed the Food Cravings Questionnaire and underwent genotyping for FTO rs9939609, the first FTO single nucleotide polymorphism associated with obesity. Compared to TT homozygotes, individuals carrying the obesity-susceptible A allele had higher total food cravings, which correlated with higher BMI. Additionally, food cravings declined with age, but this age effect differed across variants of FTO rs9939609: while TT homozygotes showed the typical age-related decline in food cravings, there was no such decline among A carriers. All subjects were scanned with [18F]fallypride PET to assess a recent proposal that at the neurochemical level FTO alters dopamine D2-like receptor (DRD2) function to influence food reward related mechanisms. However, we observed no evidence of FTO effects on DRD2 availability.
NeuroImage | 2017
Victoria Villalta-Gil; Kendra E. Hinton; Bennett A. Landman; Benjamin C. Yvernault; Scott F. Perkins; Allison S. Katsantonis; Courtney L. Sellani; Benjamin B. Lahey; David H. Zald
Abstract The amygdala (AMG) has been repeatedly implicated in the processing of threatening and negatively valenced stimuli and multiple fMRI paradigms have reported personality, genetic, and psychopathological associations with individual differences in AMG activation in these paradigms. Yet the interchangeability of activations in these probes has not been established, thus it remains unclear if we can interpret AMG responses on specific tasks as general markers of its reactivity. In this study we aimed to assess if different tasks that have been widely used within the Affective Neuroscience literature consistently recruit the AMG. Method Thirty‐two young healthy subjects completed four fMRI tasks that have all been previously shown to probe the AMG during processing of threatening stimuli: the Threat Face Matching (TFM), the Cued Aversive Picture (CAP), the Aversive and Erotica Pictures (AEP) and the Screaming Lady paradigm (SLp) tasks. Contrasts testing response to aversive stimuli relative to baseline or neutral stimuli were generated and correlations between activations in the AMG were calculated across tasks were performed for ROIs of the AMG. Results The TFM, CAP and AEP, but not the SLp, successfully recruit the AMG, among other brain regions, especially when contrasts were against baseline or nonsocial stimuli. Conjunction analysis across contrasts showed that visual cortices (VisCtx) were also consistently recruited. Correlation analysis between the extracted data for right and left AMG did not yield significant associations across tasks. By contrast, the extracted signal in VisCtx showed significant associations across tasks (range r=0.511‐r=0.630). Conclusions Three of the four paradigms revealed significant AMG reactivity, but individual differences in the magnitudes of AMG reactivity were not correlated across paradigms. By contrast, VisCtx activation appears to be a better candidate than the AMG as a measure of individual differences with convergent validity across negative emotion processing paradigms.
NeuroImage | 2016
Linh C. Dang; Gregory R. Samanez-Larkin; Jaime J. Castrellon; Scott F. Perkins; Ronald L. Cowan; David H. Zald
Psychopharmacology | 2018
Christopher T. Smith; Linh C. Dang; Leah Burgess; Scott F. Perkins; M. Danica San Juan; Darcy K. Smith; Ronald L. Cowan; Nam T. Le; Robert M. Kessler; Gregory R. Samanez-Larkin; David H. Zald
Biological Psychiatry | 2017
Kendra E. Hinton; Victoria Villalta-Gil; Scott F. Perkins; Leah Burgess; Joshua Benton; Neil D. Woodward; Bennett A. Landman; Benjamin B. Lahey; David H. Zald