Skyler S. Place
Brigham and Women's Hospital
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Skyler S. Place.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General | 2007
Jeremy M. Wolfe; Todd S. Horowitz; Michael J. Van Wert; Naomi M. Kenner; Skyler S. Place; Nour Kibbi
In visual search tasks, observers look for targets in displays containing distractors. Likelihood that targets will be missed varies with target prevalence, the frequency with which targets are presented across trials. Miss error rates are much higher at low target prevalence (1%-2%) than at high prevalence (50%). Unfortunately, low prevalence is characteristic of important search tasks such as airport security and medical screening where miss errors are dangerous. A series of experiments show this prevalence effect is very robust. In signal detection terms, the prevalence effect can be explained as a criterion shift and not a change in sensitivity. Several efforts to induce observers to adopt a better criterion fail. However, a regime of brief retraining periods with high prevalence and full feedback allows observers to hold a good criterion during periods of low prevalence with no feedback.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review | 2007
Jeremy M. Wolfe; Skyler S. Place; Todd S. Horowitz
The multiple object tracking (MOT) task has been a useful tool for studying the deployment of limited-capacity visual resources over time. Since it involves sustained attention to multiple objects, this task is a promising model for real-world visual cognition. However, real-world tasks differ in two critical ways from standard laboratory MOT designs. First, in real-world tracking, it is unusual for the set of tracked items to be identified all at once and to remain unchanged over time. Second, real-world tracking tasks may need to be sustained over a period of minutes, and not mere seconds. How well is MOT performance maintained over extended periods of time? In four experiments, we demonstrate that observers can dynamically “juggle” objects in and out of the tracked set with little apparent cost, and can sustain this performance for up to 10 min at a time. This performance requires implicit or explicit feedback. In the absence of feedback, performance tracking drops steadily over the course of several minutes.
Psychological Science | 2009
Skyler S. Place; Peter M. Todd; Lars Penke; Jens B. Asendorpf
The ability to judge another individuals romantic interest level—both toward oneself and toward others—is an adaptively important skill when choosing a suitable mate to pursue. We tested this ability using videos of individuals on speed dates as stimuli. Male and female observers were equally good at predicting interest levels, but they were more accurate when predicting male interest: Predictions of female interest were just above chance. Observers predicted interest successfully using stimuli as short as 10 s, and they performed best when watching clips of the middle or end of the speed date. There was considerable variability between daters, with some being very easy to read and others apparently masking their true intentions. Variability between observers was also found. The results suggest that the ability to read nonverbal behavior quickly in mate choice is present not only for individuals in the interaction, but also for third-party observers.
Journal of Vision | 2010
Skyler S. Place; Todd S. Horowitz
2 3 4 1 # of Targets (70% accuracy threshold = 30°) Observers have good resolution for target direction Purpose / Overview Purpose/Overview Multiple object tracking (MOT) experiments have demonstrated that observers can track multiple independently moving objects (Cavanagh & Alvarez, 2005; Pylyshyn, 2001). We introduce a new method to demonstrate that observers are tracking target trajectories, not merely locations. Do you have better directional information for targets than non-targets?
Journal of Vision | 2010
Todd S. Horowitz; Skyler S. Place
template references Alvarez, G. A., Horowitz, T. S., Arsenio, H. C., DiMase, J. S., & Wolfe, J. M. (in press). Do multielement visual tracking and visual search draw continuously on the same visual attention resources? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance. Fencsik, D. E., Horowitz, T. S., Klieger, S. B., & Wolfe, J. M. (2004). Target reacquisition strategies in multiple object tracking, Vision Sciences Society (Vol. 4, pp. 77). Horowitz, T. S., Birnkrant, R. S., Wolfe, J. M., Tran, L., & Fencsik, D. E. (2004). Tracking invisible objects, Vision Sciences Society (Vol. 4, pp. 76). Keane, B. P., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2004). Tracking behind occluders is not based on predicting likely reappearance locations. Journal of Vision, 4(8), 362a.
Journal of Vision | 2010
David E. Fencsik; Skyler S. Place; Jeremy M. Wolfe; Todd S. Horowitz
Dick, M., Ullman, S., & Sagi, D. (1987). Parallel and serial processes in motion detection. Science, 237, 400–402. Ivry, R. B., & Cohen, A. (1992). Asymmetry in visual search for targets defined by differences in movement speed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 1045–1057. Rosenholtz, R. (1999). A simple saliency model predicts a number of motion popout phenomena. Vision Research, 39, 3157–3163. Rosenholtz, R. (2001). Search asymmetries? What search asymmetries? Perception & Psychophysics, 63(3), 476–489.
Evolution and Human Behavior | 2010
Skyler S. Place; Peter M. Todd; Lars Penke; Jens B. Asendorpf
Behavioral Ecology | 2012
Robert I. Bowers; Skyler S. Place; Peter M. Todd; Lars Penke; Jens B. Asendorpf
Journal of Vision | 2010
David E. Fencsik; Todd S. Horowitz; Skyler S. Place; Sarah B. Klieger; Jeremy M. Wolfe
Evolution and Human Behavior | 2012
Skyler S. Place; Peter M. Todd; Jinying Zhuang; Lars Penke; Jens B. Asendorpf