Stephanie A. McMains
Princeton University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Stephanie A. McMains.
The Journal of Neuroscience | 2007
Jascha D. Swisher; Mark A. Halko; Lotfi B. Merabet; Stephanie A. McMains; David C. Somers
Human parietal cortex is implicated in a wide variety of sensory and cognitive functions, yet its precise organization remains unclear. Visual field maps provide a potential structural basis for descriptions of functional organization. Here, we detail the topography of a series of five maps of the contralateral visual hemifield within human posterior parietal cortex. These maps are located along the medial bank of the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and are revealed by direct visual stimulation during functional magnetic resonance imaging, allowing these parietal regions to be routinely and reliably identified simultaneously with occipital visual areas. Two of these maps (IPS3 and IPS4) are novel, whereas two others (IPS1 and IPS2) have previously been revealed only by higher-order cognitive tasks. Area V7, a previously identified visual map, is observed to lie within posterior IPS and to share a foveal representation with IPS1. These parietal maps are reliably observed across scan sessions; however, their precise topography varies between individuals. The multimodal organization of posterior IPS mirrors this variability in visual topography, with complementary tactile activations found immediately adjacent to the visual maps both medially and laterally. These visual maps may provide a practical framework in which to characterize the functional organization of human IPS.
The Journal of Neuroscience | 2009
Michael Arcaro; Stephanie A. McMains; Benjamin D. Singer; Sabine Kastner
Functional magnetic resonance imaging studies have shown that human ventral visual cortex anterior to human visual area V4 contains two visual field maps, VO-1 and VO-2, that together form the ventral occipital (VO) cluster (Brewer et al., 2005). This cluster is characterized by common functional response properties and responds preferentially to color and object stimuli. Here, we confirm the topographic and functional characteristics of the VO cluster and describe two new visual field maps that are located anterior to VO-2 extending across the collateral sulcus into the posterior parahippocampal cortex (PHC). We refer to these visual field maps as parahippocampal areas PHC-1 and PHC-2. Each PHC map contains a topographic representation of contralateral visual space. The polar angle representation in PHC-1 extends from regions near the lower vertical meridian (that is the shared border with VO-2) to those close to the upper vertical meridian (that is the shared border with PHC-2). The polar angle representation in PHC-2 is a mirror reversal of the PHC-1 representation. PHC-1 and PHC-2 share a foveal representation and show a strong bias toward representations of peripheral eccentricities. Both the foveal and peripheral representations of PHC-1 and PHC-2 respond more strongly to scenes than to objects or faces, with greater scene preference in PHC-2 than PHC-1. Importantly, both areas heavily overlap with the functionally defined parahippocampal place area. Our results suggest that ventral visual cortex can be subdivided on the basis of topographic criteria into a greater number of discrete maps than previously thought.
The Journal of Neuroscience | 2011
Stephanie A. McMains; Sabine Kastner
Multiple stimuli present in the visual field at the same time compete for neural representation by mutually suppressing their evoked activity throughout visual cortex, providing a neural correlate for the limited processing capacity of the visual system. Competitive interactions among stimuli can be counteracted by top-down, goal-directed mechanisms such as attention, and by bottom-up, stimulus-driven mechanisms. Because these two processes cooperate in everyday life to bias processing toward behaviorally relevant or particularly salient stimuli, it has proven difficult to study interactions between top-down and bottom-up mechanisms. Here, we used an experimental paradigm in which we first isolated the effects of a bottom-up influence on neural competition by parametrically varying the degree of perceptual grouping in displays that were not attended. Second, we probed the effects of directed attention on the competitive interactions induced with the parametric design. We found that the amount of attentional modulation varied linearly with the degree of competition left unresolved by bottom-up processes, such that attentional modulation was greatest when neural competition was little influenced by bottom-up mechanisms and smallest when competition was strongly influenced by bottom-up mechanisms. These findings suggest that the strength of attentional modulation in the visual system is constrained by the degree to which competitive interactions have been resolved by bottom-up processes related to the segmentation of scenes into candidate objects.
The Journal of Neuroscience | 2005
Stephanie A. McMains; David C. Somers
Many visual tasks require deployment of attention to multiple objects or locations. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioral experiments to investigate the relative processing efficiency of two putative attentional mechanisms for performing such tasks: the “zoom lens” and “multiple spotlights.” Two key questions were investigated: (1) does splitting the spotlight into multiple foci incur an overhead cost that diminishes the efficacy of attention compared with the zoom lens, and (2) does splitting the spotlight provide a benefit relative to the zoom lens by conserving attention resources that otherwise would be directed to task irrelevant stimuli? For both mechanisms, attending to multiple object locations decreased processing efficiency at a single location, resulting in both decreased behavioral performance and decreased blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal attentional modulation. When the two mechanisms attended to multiple objects across the same spatial extent, the multiple spotlight mechanism, which ignores intervening stimuli, yielded better performance and higher BOLD signal. When the two mechanisms processed the same number of stimuli, splitting the spotlight neither impaired performance nor diminished BOLD signal in occipital cortex. The surprising efficiency of the multiple spotlight mechanism supports the emerging view that spatial attention is easily deployed in a diverse range of spatial configurations.
Neuropsychologia | 2009
Hagit Magen; Tatiana-Aloi Emmanouil; Stephanie A. McMains; Sabine Kastner; Anne Treisman
Limits to the capacity of visual short-term memory (VSTM) indicate a maximum storage of only 3 or 4 items. Recently, it has been suggested that activity in a specific part of the brain, the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), is correlated with behavioral estimates of VSTM capacity and might reflect a capacity-limited store. In three experiments that varied the delay period and the stimuli to be stored, we found dissociations between functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) activity in PPC and behavioral measures of capacity. When the delay length increased, fMRI activity in this area increased with memory load beyond the behaviorally determined limits of capacity. The results suggest that activity in PPC may reflect the attentional demands of short-term memory rehearsal processes rather than capacity limitations, and imply that a larger number of items than that determined by behavioral measures of capacity may be rehearsed during STM tasks. This account is consistent with the role of PPC in attentional processes and with the close correlation between brain areas that are involved in attention and those that mediate STM.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | 2010
Stephanie A. McMains; Sabine Kastner
Multiple stimuli that are present simultaneously in the visual field compete for neural representation. At the same time, however, multiple stimuli in cluttered scenes also undergo perceptual organization according to certain rules originally defined by the Gestalt psychologists such as similarity or proximity, thereby segmenting scenes into candidate objects. How can these two seemingly orthogonal neural processes that occur early in the visual processing stream be reconciled? One possibility is that competition occurs among perceptual groups rather than at the level of elements within a group. We probed this idea using fMRI by assessing competitive interactions across visual cortex in displays containing varying degrees of perceptual organization or perceptual grouping (Grp). In strong Grp displays, elements were arranged such that either an illusory figure or a group of collinear elements were present, whereas in weak Grp displays the same elements were arranged randomly. Competitive interactions among stimuli were overcome throughout early visual cortex and V4, when elements were grouped regardless of Grp type. Our findings suggest that context-dependent grouping mechanisms and competitive interactions are linked to provide a bottom–up bias toward candidate objects in cluttered scenes.
Cerebral Cortex | 2016
Mark C. Eldaief; Stephanie A. McMains; R. Matthew Hutchison; Mark A. Halko; Alvaro Pascual-Leone
&NA; Communication between cortical regions is necessary for optimal cognitive processing. Functional relationships between cortical regions can be inferred through measurements of temporal synchrony in spontaneous activity patterns. These relationships can be further elaborated by surveying effects of cortical lesions upon inter‐regional connectivity. Lesions to cortical hubs and heteromodal association regions are expected to induce distributed connectivity changes and higher‐order cognitive deficits, yet their functional consequences remain relatively unexplored. Here, we used resting‐state fMRI to investigate intrinsic functional connectivity (FC) and graph theoretical metrics in 12 patients with circumscribed lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) portion of the Default Network (DN), and compared these metrics with those observed in healthy matched comparison participants and a sample of 1139 healthy individuals. Despite significant mPFC destruction, patients did not demonstrate weakened intrinsic FC among undamaged DN nodes. Instead, network‐specific changes were manifested as weaker negative correlations between the DN and attentional and somatomotor networks. These findings conflict with the DN being a homogenous system functionally anchored at mPFC. Rather, they implicate a role for mPFC in mediating cross‐network functional interactions. More broadly, our data suggest that lesions to association cortical hubs might induce clinical deficits by disrupting communication between interacting large‐scale systems.
Journal of Vision | 2015
Stephanie A. McMains; R. Matthew Hutchison; Ross William Mair
Slice-accelerated EPI using multiband (MB) RF pulses that allow for simultaneous multi-slice (SMS) acquisition of BOLD contrast images can significantly enhance the temporal and spatial resolution of fMRI by acquiring up to 8 non-contiguous slices at the same time, thus enabling whole-brain sub-second TRs. Here we studied visual cortex response at a variety of MB accelerations and TR reductions to investigate whether there were any costs associated with parameters that allowed for whole-brain, sub-second data collection at 2mm resolution. 6 subjects were scanned (3.0T Siemens Tim Trio) with a 32-ch head coil while subjects performed a fixation task and blocks of flashing checkerboards were presented to alternating visual fields. BOLD scans were acquired at 3mm and 2mm isotropic resolutions, a max TR of 3s, and MB accelerations of 0 (conventional BOLD sequence), 1, 4 and 8 (Siemens WIP 770A). Beta and t-statistics were extracted from regions localized in visual cortex. With a TR of 3s, there were 91 timepoints, while for TR= 1.25/0.75/0.7s, there were 184/307/328 timepoints. There were no significant differences in betas for any parameters, or in t-statistics for levels of MB when holding the TR constant. Shortening the TR increased t-statistics significantly. This advantage was reduced when temporal autocorrelations in the noise were modeled. An event-related study was also conducted for 2mm voxels to compare 3s TR (MB1) versus 750ms TR (MB8). Betas were larger for the MB8 scans, likely due to improved characterization of the hemodynamic response, even though stimulus onset was jittered to the TR. The results suggest that whole brain coverage with high spatial and temporal resolution can be achieved using SMS with little to no cost in terms of BOLD signal sensitivity, as measured by betas and t-statistics, even though time-series SNR decreased significantly at high MB factors. Meeting abstract presented at VSS 2015.
Neurobiology of Attention | 2005
David C. Somers; Stephanie A. McMains
Functional MRI and visual psychophysics were employed to investigate space-based attentional selection mechanisms in human occipital cortex. Our 1999 findings, along with nearly simultaneous findings from other laboratories, demonstrated that spatially specific attention could operate robustly even in primary visual cortex. Spatial deployment of attention operates in a “push-pull” fashion, both increasing responses at attended locations and decreasing responses at nonattended locations. Parametric studies suggest that spatial attention acts primarily as an “additive bias” signal, whose amplitude is largely independent of stimulus strength. Finally, the spatial window of attention, as reflected in retinotopic cortical activation, is highly flexible and it may be split into multiple “spotlights,” if the task so demands.
Neuron | 2004
Stephanie A. McMains; David C. Somers