Stuart C. Church
University of Bristol
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Stuart C. Church.
Advances in The Study of Behavior | 2000
Innes C. Cuthill; Julian C. Partridge; Andrew T. D. Bennett; Stuart C. Church; Nathan S. Hart; Sarah Hunt
Publisher Summary Birds can see ultraviolet (UV) light because, unlike humans, their lenses and other ocular media transmit UV, and they possess a class of photoreceptor, which is maximally sensitive to violet or UV light, depending on the species. Birds have a tetrachromatic color space, as compared to the trichromacy of humans. Birds, along with some reptiles and fish, also possess double cones in large numbers and a cone class. This chapter discusses a range of behavioral experiments, from several species, which show that UV information is utilized in behavioral decisions, notably in foraging and signaling. Removal of UV wavelengths affects mate choice even in species that are colorful to humans. These studies emphasize that avian and human color perceptions are different and that the use of human color standards, and even artificial lighting, may produce misleading results. However, genuinely objective measures of color are available, as are, importantly, models for mapping the measured spectra into an avian color space.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London series B-biological sciences | 1998
Stuart C. Church; Andrew T. D. Bennett; Innes C. Cuthill; Julian C. Partridge
The function of avian ultraviolet (UV) vision is only just beginning to be understood. One plausible hypothesis is that UV vision enhances the foraging ability of birds. To test this, we carried out behavioural experiments using wild–caught blue tits foraging for cabbage moth and winter moth caterpillars on natural and artificial backgrounds. The light environment in our experiments was manipulated using either UV–blocking or UV–transmitting filters. We found that the blue tits tended to find the first prey item (out of four) more quickly when UV cues were present. This suggests that UV vision offers benefits to birds when searching for cryptic prey, despite the prey and backgrounds reflecting relatively little UV. Although there was no direct effect of UV on the time taken to find all four prey items in a trial, search performance in the absence of UV wavelengths tended to increase over the course of an experiment. This may reflect changes in the search tactics of the birds. To our knowledge, these are the first data to suggest that birds use UV cues to detect cryptic insect prey, and have implications for our understanding of protective coloration.
Acta Ethologica | 2000
Innes C. Cuthill; Nathan S. Hart; Julian C. Partridge; Andrew T. D. Bennett; Sarah Hunt; Stuart C. Church
Abstract Video playback potentially allows the presentation, manipulation, and replication of realistic moving visual stimuli, in a way that is impossible with real animals or static dummies, and difficult even with mechanical models. However, there are special problems attached to the use of this technology; this article concentrates on the problem of accurate colour rendition. Video and television simulate the colour of objects rather than reproduce the spectrum of light that they naturally emit, transmit, or reflect. This simulation is achieved by using relatively narrow waveband light to stimulate the cone cells in the retina in a similar pattern to that produced by the natural object. However, species differ in the spectral tuning of their photoreceptors, so a faithful colour rendition for a human is unlikely to be achieved for another species. This problem is discussed with special reference to birds, a taxon renown for its colourfulness and frequent use in behavioural experiments but which has a very different colour vision from that of humans. We stress that the major pitfalls that can arise when using video playback with avian subjects can also occur in ’normal’ behavioural experiments. However, the problems of faithful colour rendition are particularly severe with video, and the major benefits that the technology brings will only be realised under a limited range of circumstances, with careful validation experiments.
Animal Behaviour | 2003
Elizabeth M. White; Julian C. Partridge; Stuart C. Church
Abstract Some fish, including the guppy, have the ability to perceive ultraviolet (UV) wavelengths. Female guppies prefer to associate with males that are viewed under light conditions that include UV-A, in preference to conditions lacking these wavelengths. We used reflexion spectrophotometry to show that male guppies reflect UV light from both their structural (purple, green and white) and pigment (orange) colour patches and that males differ in the levels of UV light reflected. Varying components of UV may affect both the brightness and hue of particular colour patches. This may produce nonspectral colours that are visible to the guppy but that are outside human perception. We used video analysis to quantify male reflexion in the UV and visible wavebands. Male guppies with high and low UV reflexion, but similar human-visible coloration and area of coloration, were paired for use in mate choice experiments. Female guppies shown pairs of males differing in their levels of UV reflexion had no preference for either high or low UV reflexion. This suggests that UV reflexion does not provide particularly significant information relating to male quality or influence female preference in this population of guppies. Copyright 2003 Published by Elsevier Science Ltd on behalf of The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour
Oecologia | 1997
Stuart C. Church; Michael Jowers; John A. Allen
Abstract Despite the fact that the vast majority of natural prey items are dispersed in a non-random manner, few studies of frequency-dependent selective predation have explicitly examined the effect of prey dispersion on selectivity. We examined the effect of prey dispersion on the direction and strength of frequency-dependent selection by wild birds feeding on artificial prey (green or brown pastry baits). In a series of four experimental manipulations, we tested for the occurrence of frequency dependence with two different dispersion patterns (random or clumped). Manipulations were carried out at one of two absolute densities (25 prey m−2 or 100 prey m−2), and were repeated at different sites in Southampton, England and Aljarafe, Spain. Our results suggest that prey dispersion has no effect on either frequency-dependent or -independent preferences. One possible explanation for this is that the birds had virtually complete information about prey frequencies at the relatively high densities used and based their preferences on their overall perception of availability. However, we cannot rule out the possibility that dispersion may influence selectivity when prey are cryptic or available at lower absolute densities. Although there was no effect of dispersion, frequency-dependent selection was, overall, significantly anti-apostatic (i.e. rare baits were more preferred than common baits). This anti-apostatic effect was stronger in Southampton than Aljarafe and stronger at 100 prey m−2 than 25 prey m−2. The differences in the strength of selection between the two locations was interpreted in terms of (i) whether the avian predators foraged in flocks or not, and (ii) the number of different species present (and, consequently, the variation in preference among individual birds).
Animal Behaviour | 2005
Elizabeth M. White; Stuart C. Church; Laura J. Willoughby; Sarah J. Hudson; Julian C. Partridge
The spectral composition of the light environment can have important implications for visually mediated behaviours. We examined how spectral irradiance influences the behaviour of guppies foraging for live zooplankton prey. Daphnia are semitransparent, transmitting human-visible wavelengths but absorbing strongly in the ultraviolet (UV). We first tested the hypothesis that UV wavelengths contribute to foraging efficiency. We then used lighting conditions that selectively blocked regions of the spectrum (UV, short-wavelength, medium-wavelength and long-wavelength light) to determine the effect of removal of these particular spectral regions on guppy foraging rate. Guppies foraged equally effectively under UV-present and UV-absent conditions, suggesting that UV wavelengths are not particularly important for detecting and locating prey under our experimental conditions. Similarly, foraging effectiveness was not significantly affected by the removal of short-wavelength information. In the absence of long wavelengths, however, foraging rate was significantly reduced. This suggests that long-wavelength information is particularly important in this foraging task. We used image analysis to measure the contrast of Daphnia against the background. Contrast was reduced in the absence of UV or long wavelengths but increased when short wavelengths were removed. Variation in contrast cannot, however, fully account for the results observed. It is also possible that the removal of long wavelengths hindered other factors such as motion perception.
Journal of Insect Behavior | 1999
Thomas N. Sherratt; Sarah E. Ruff; Stuart C. Church
Kin selection theory suggests that cannibalism is more likely to spread and be maintained if cannibalism of close relatives can be preferentially avoided. One important group of insects in which kin discrimination might be expected to evolve is cannibalistic tree-hole mosquitoes. Larvae of these species develop in small, ephemeral water bodies, where they regularly encounter both relatives and nonrelatives. In this study we compared the degree of sib cannibalism with the degree of nonsib cannibalism in two ecologically distinct mosquito species: an anautogenous (blood-feeding) detritivore Trichoprosopon digitatum (Culicinae) and an autogenous predator Toxorhynchites moctezuma (Toxorhynchitinae). Despite a wealth of literature documenting kin-biased discrimination in other insects, neither of these tree-hold mosquito species preferentially consumed nonrelatives. In the case of Tr. digitatum, the size-dependent nature of larval cannibalism may preclude additional selection for preferential consumption of nonrelatives, but in the autogenous Tx. moctezuma the direct nutritive and indirect competitive benefits of indiscriminate cannibalism may outweigh the immediate costs of consuming a relative.
The Journal of Experimental Biology | 2001
Sarah Hunt; Innes C. Cuthill; Andrew T. D. Bennett; Stuart C. Church; Julian C. Partridge
Behavioral Ecology | 2002
Elizabeth J. Smith; Julian C. Partridge; Kn Parsons; Elizabeth M. White; Innes C. Cuthill; Andrew T. D. Bennett; Stuart C. Church
The Journal of Experimental Biology | 2001
Sam A. Maddocks; Stuart C. Church; Innes C. Cuthill