Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Sue J. Kang is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Sue J. Kang.


Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology | 2004

Caregiver-Administered Neuropsychiatric Inventory (CGA-NPI):

Sue J. Kang; Seong Hye Choi; Byung Hwa Lee; Yong Jeong; Dong Seok Hahm; Il Woo Han; Jeffrey L. Cummings; Duk L. Na

The Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) is used to assess neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia patients. To reduce clinicians’ time taken to administer the NPI, the authors studied a caregiver-administered NPI (CGA-NPI), in which caregivers completed the written form of the NPI worksheet. After a brief presupervision session, the caregivers of 61 dementia patients completed the CGA-NPI by reading through the worksheet. This was followed by a postsupervision session to check if the caregivers had completed the form appropriately. The correlation between the prevalence rates of each neuropsychiatric symptom obtained by the CGA-NPI and those obtained by the NPI was fair to good (. = 0.57-0.90) in all domains. All frequency, severity, and caregivers’ distress scores of the CGA-NPI correlated significantly with those of the NPI (r> 0.6, P< .001). Total CGA-NPI scores also correlated highly with total NPI scores (r= 0.86, P< .001). These results suggest that the CGA-NPI can substitute for the NPI, saving administration time.


Neuropsychologia | 2004

The Character-line Bisection Task: a new test for hemispatial neglect.

Byung Hwa Lee; Sue J. Kang; J.M. Park; Young-Ik Son; Kyung Han Lee; John C. Adair; Kenneth M. Heilman; Duk L. Na

A failure to report or respond to stimuli presented in a portion of space is termed hemispatial neglect. Line bisection and line cancellation are two of the tests used most commonly to assess for neglect. Perhaps, because neglect can be induced by a variety of deficits, neither of these tests used alone is as sensitive as both used together. Hence, the primary purpose of this study was to assess the sensitivity, reliability and validity of a new test called the Character-line Bisection Task (CLBT) that combines features of both the bisection and cancellation tests. Since local attention and language are primarily mediated by and activate the left hemisphere, our second goal was to learn if the CLBT and especially the letter version induce a greater rightward bias than the solid-line bisection task. Eighty patients with acute right hemisphere stroke and 81 controls performed the CLBT that consists of two subtests, the Letter-line and Star-line Bisection tasks. All subjects also completed four conventional tests for neglect (Standard solid-line bisection, line cancellation, Star Cancellation, and figure copying). In the bisection tasks both patients and controls bisected to the right with the CLBT than with the solid-line bisection task, suggesting the CLBT induces asymmetrical hemispheric activation. This enhanced rightward deviation with the CLBT was the same for the Letter-line and Star-line Bisection tasks. In regard to sensitivity, we defined the presence of neglect syndrome based on a total score derived from performance of controls on all six tests. This total score detected 55 (68.8%) patients with neglect. Within this group, the Letter-line and Star-line tasks diagnosed neglect in 50 and 48 patients, respectively, resulting in the highest sensitivities (90.9, 87.3%) of the six tests. Thus, the CLBTs demonstrated higher sensitivities than the other commonly used neglect tests and these new tests can be useful for the detection and quantification of unilateral neglect.


Neurobiology of Aging | 2013

Changes in subcortical structures in early- versus late-onset Alzheimer's disease

Hanna Cho; Sang Won Seo; Jeong Hun Kim; Changsoo Kim; Byoung Seok Ye; Geon Ha Kim; Young Noh; Hee-Jin Kim; Cindy W. Yoon; Joon Kyung Seong; Chang Hun Kim; Sue J. Kang; Juhee Chin; Sung Tae Kim; Kyung Han Lee; Duk L. Na

Patients with early-onset Alzheimers disease (EOAD) are reported to be different from those with late-onset Alzheimers disease (LOAD) in terms of neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings. In this study, we aimed to compare the longitudinal volume changes of 6 subcortical structures (the amygdala, hippocampus, thalamus, putamen, globus pallidus, and caudate nucleus) between patients with EOAD and LOAD for 3 years. We prospectively recruited 36 patients with probable Alzheimers disease (14 EOAD, 22 LOAD) and 14 normal control subjects. We analyzed the volume of subcortical structures using an automatic surface-based method. At baseline, there were no differences in the volumes of subcortical structures between patients with EOAD and LOAD. However, over 3 years of longitudinal follow-up, patients with EOAD showed more rapid volumetric decline in the caudate, putamen, and thalamus than patients with LOAD, which is consistent with neuropsychological results. Our findings suggested that the cognitive reserve theory might be applicable to explain different decline rates of the volumes of the basal ganglia and thalamus according to onset age.


Neurology | 2005

Pattern of glucose hypometabolism in frontotemporal dementia with motor neuron disease

Yong Jeong; Kyu-Hyun Park; Sang Soo Cho; Eun-Joo Kim; Sue J. Kang; Se Kim; Eun-Suk Kang; Duk L. Na

Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) often coexists with motor neuron disease (MND). To characterize glucose hypometabolism in patients with FTD with MND (FTD/MND), the authors compared the glucose metabolism of 8 patients with FTD/MND with that of 29 patients with FTD. All of the patients with FTD/MND showed glucose hypometabolism only in the frontal area, whereas most patients with FTD had hypometabolism in the frontal and temporal areas. FTD/MND also showed a more symmetric pattern of glucose hypometabolism than FTD.


Neurobiology of Aging | 2013

Longitudinal changes of cortical thickness in early- versus late-onset Alzheimer's disease

Hanna Cho; Seun Jeon; Sue J. Kang; Jong-Min Lee; Jae-Hong Lee; Geon Ha Kim; Ji Soo Shin; Chi Hun Kim; Young Noh; Kiho Im; Sung Tae Kim; Juhee Chin; Sang Won Seo; Duk L. Na

Early-onset Alzheimers disease (EOAD) has been shown to progress more rapidly than late-onset Alzheimers disease (LOAD). However, no studies have compared the topography of brain volume reduction over time. The purpose of this 3-year longitudinal study was to compare EOAD and LOAD in terms of their rates of decline in cognitive testing and topography of cortical thinning. We prospectively recruited 36 patients with AD (14 EOAD and 22 LOAD) and 14 normal controls. All subjects were assessed with neuropsychological tests and with magnetic resonance imaging at baseline, Year 1, and Year 3. The EOAD group showed more rapid decline than the LOAD group in attention, language, and frontal-executive tests. The EOAD group also showed more rapid cortical thinning in widespread association cortices. In contrast, the LOAD group presented more rapid cortical thinning than the EOAD group only in the left parahippocampal gyrus. Our study suggests that patients with EOAD show more rapid cortical atrophy than patients with LOAD, which accounts for faster cognitive decline on neuropsychological tests.


Stroke | 2002

Hypertensive Pontine Microhemorrhage

Jee-Hyang Jeong; Soo Jin Yoon; Sue J. Kang; Kyung Gyu Choi; Duk L. Na

Background and Purpose— This study investigated whether the topography of hypertensive pontine microhemorrhages (hPMHs) resembles that of larger primary pontine hemorrhages. Methods— Sixty-nine consecutive patients with small-vessel disease underwent imaging with gradient-echo MRI, and 27 patients with hPMH were detected. Lesion size and location along the rostrocaudal (longitudinal), lateral (coronal), and anteroposterior (sagittal) axes were determined. Results— A total of 52 hPMHs were identified in the 27 patients (mean, 1.93±2.4 per patient). The lesions showed a nonrandom distribution, with a propensity to occur in the middle pons in the rostrocaudal axis, posterior half of the basis pontis in the anteroposterior axis, and central subdivision within the lateral axis. The area of hPMH ranged from 1.3 to 19.0 mm2 (mean, 5.06±3.72 mm2). The size of hPMH did not vary as a function of lesion location. Conclusions— Previous studies reported that primary pontine hemorrhages tend to occur in the middle pons and at the junction of basis pontis and tegmentum. Therefore, topographical correspondences between large and small pontine hemorrhages may provide evidence that the 2 lesions share some etiological basis. Further investigation may determine whether hPMHs portend future symptomatic primary pontine hemorrhages.


Neurocase | 2004

Mechanism of the Closing-in Phenomenon in a Figure Copying Task in Alzheimer’s Disease Patients

Byung Hwa Lee; Juhee Chin; Sue J. Kang; Eun-Joo Kim; Key Chung Park; Duk L. Na

Abstract The “closing-in phenomenon” in figure copying tasks refers to a tendency to copy near the target, or to overlap the target to be copied. The mechanisms underlying the closing-in phenomenon have not been fully elucidated. We posit that closing-in may be related to the patients’ compensatory strategies to overcome visuospatial dysfunction or visuospatial working memory deficit. Thus, it is expected that as the complexity of the target figure or the distance from the target to the copying space is increased, the magnitude of closing-in will be increased. Thirteen patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) who demonstrated closing-in on a screening test and 15 healthy controls participated in this study. Each subject copied figures in conditions that varied in terms of figure complexity and distance from the target to the copying space. Neither figure complexity nor distance between the target and copying space affected the degree of closing-in in normal subjects. In contrast, in AD patients, the magnitude of closing-in increased as a function of figure complexity; however closing-in was unchanged by varying the distance from the target to the copying space. Our results suggest that copying near the target figure might be the patients’ strategy to compensate for their visuospatial dysfunction or visuospatial working memory deficits.


Neurobiology of Aging | 2013

The effects of small vessel disease and amyloid burden on neuropsychiatric symptoms: A study among patients with subcortical vascular cognitive impairments

Hee-Jin Kim; Sue J. Kang; Changsoo Kim; Geon Ha Kim; Seun Jeon; Jong-Min Lee; Seung Jun Oh; Jae Seung Kim; Yearn Seong Choe; Kyung Han Lee; Young Noh; Hanna Cho; Cindy W. Yoon; Juhee Chin; Jeffrey L. Cummings; Jae-Hong Lee; Duk L. Na; Sang Won Seo

Neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) affect the quality of life of patients with dementia and increase the burden on caregivers. We aimed to evaluate how small vessel disease (SVD) such as lacunae or white matter hyperintensities (WMH), and amyloid burden affect NPS. We recruited 127 patients with subcortical vascular cognitive impairment who were assessed with brain magnetic resonance imaging, Pittsburgh compound-B (PiB) positron emission tomography and the neuropsychiatric inventory (NPI). To explore the association between lacunae, WMH, or PiB retention ratio and NPS, we performed multivariate regression analysis after controlling for possible confounders. Each additional lacuna, especially in the frontal region, was associated with higher odds of depression, apathy, aberrant motor behavior, nighttime behavior, appetite changes, and higher score of total NPI; larger WMH volume, especially in the frontal region, was associated with higher odds of apathy and higher score of total NPI. Furthermore, for the effects of lacunae or WMH on total NPI score we set Clinical Dementia Rating Sum of Boxes as the mediator. Greater PiB retention ratio was associated with higher odds of delusions and irritability. The SVD and amyloid pathologies did not show interactive effects on NPS. Our findings suggested that SVD and amyloid burden independently affected specific NPS.


Journal of Alzheimer's Disease | 2014

Shape Changes of the Basal Ganglia and Thalamus in Alzheimer's Disease: A Three-Year Longitudinal Study

Hanna Cho; Jeong Hun Kim; Changsoo Kim; Byoung Seok Ye; Hee-Jin Kim; Cindy W. Yoon; Young Noh; Geon Ha Kim; Yeo Jin Kim; Jung-Hyun Kim; Chang Hun Kim; Sue J. Kang; Juhee Chin; Sung Tae Kim; Kyung Han Lee; Duk L. Na; Joon Kyung Seong; Sang Won Seo

BACKGROUND A large number of Alzheimers disease (AD) studies have focused on medial temporal and cortical atrophy, while changes in the basal ganglia or thalamus have received less attention. OBJECTIVE The aim of this study was to investigate the existence of progressive topographical shape changes in the basal ganglia (caudate nucleus, putamen, and globus pallidus) and thalamus concurrent with AD disease progression over three years. This study also examined whether declines in volumes of the basal ganglia or thalamus might be responsible for cognitive decline in patients with AD. METHODS Thirty-six patients with early stage AD and 14 normal control subjects were prospectively recruited for this study. All subjects were assessed with neuropsychological tests and MRI at baseline and Years 1 and 3. A longitudinal shape analysis of the basal ganglia and thalamus was performed by employing a boundary surface-based shape analysis method. RESULTS AD patients exhibited specific regional atrophy in the right caudate nucleus and the bilateral putamen at baseline, and as the disease progressed, regional atrophic changes in the left caudate nucleus were found to conform to a distinct topography after controlling the total brain volume. Volumetric decline of the caudate nucleus and putamen correlated with cognitive decline in frontal function after controlling for age, gender, education, follow-up years, and total brain volume changes. CONCLUSION Our findings suggest that shape changes of the basal ganglia occurred regardless of whole brain atrophy as AD progressed and were also responsible for cognitive decline that was observed from the frontal function tests.


Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology | 2005

Pseudoneglect in solid-line versus character-line bisection tasks: A test for attention dominance theory

Byung Hwa Lee; Minjee Kim; Sue J. Kang; Key Chung Park; Eun-Joo Kim; John C. Adair; Duk L. Na

ObjectiveNormal subjects tend to bisect lines slightly to the left of the true midpoint, a phenomenon termed pseudoneglect. To test whether pseudoneglect relates to the right hemisphere’s dominance for spatial attention or to the hemispheric difference in processing global-local stimulus properties, we administered conventional solid-line (SBT) and novel character-line (CBT) bisection tasks to normal subjects of different ages. MethodsNormal subjects, consisting of 40 young and 40 older individuals, received 3 experimental tasks, a standard SBT and 2 types of CBT. Each subject completed 10 consecutive trials of each task presented in counterbalanced order between subjects. ResultsAcross age groups, deviations on CBT were further to the right than those of SBT, and the leftward bias (pseudoneglect) was significant only in SBT. ConclusionThese results indicate that the bisection errors in normal subjects depend on the characteristics demanded by the specific task. Thus, our findings argue against the attention dominance theory and support a “task specificity” theory for pseudoneglect.

Collaboration


Dive into the Sue J. Kang's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Duk L. Na

Samsung Medical Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Juhee Chin

Samsung Medical Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Eun-Joo Kim

Pusan National University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Geon Ha Kim

Ewha Womans University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge