Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Susan F. te Pas is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Susan F. te Pas.


Vision Research | 2006

Adaptive center-surround interactions in human vision revealed during binocular rivalry

Chris L. E. Paffen; Duje Tadin; Susan F. te Pas; Randolph Blake; Frans A. J. Verstraten

We used binocular rivalry as a psychophysical probe to explore center-surround interactions in orientation, motion and color processing. Addition of the surround matching one of the rival targets dramatically altered rivalry dynamics. For all visual sub-modalities tested, predominance of the high-contrast rival target matched to the surround was greatly reduced-a result that disappeared at low contrast. At low contrast, addition of the surround boosted dominance of orientation and motion targets matched to the surround. This contrast-dependent modulation of center-surround interactions seems to be a general property of the visual system and may reflect an adaptive balance between surround suppression and spatial summation.


applied perception in graphics and visualization | 2005

A comparison of material and illumination discrimination performance for real rough, real smooth and computer generated smooth spheres

Susan F. te Pas; Sylvia C. Pont

The appearance of objects in natural scenes is determined by their reflectance, their 3D texture, their shape and by the nature of the illumination. Results of previous experiments using computer generated images of spheres with different reflectance modes and under different canonical illuminations suggested that perception of reflectance mode and illumination are basically confounded. In the present study we investigate whether the conclusions from the experiments with simplified rendered spheres can be extended to ecologically valid images. We use two sets of photographs of real spheres, the first set is taken from the Dror Database [Dror et al. 2001], with simple reflectance modes but complex natural illumination (e.g. desklamp, lab, foodcourt). The second set is taken from the Utrecht Oranges Database [Pont and Koenderink 2003], with simple canonical illumination but material consisting of both reflectance and 3D texture differences (e.g. orange, golf ball, christmas decoration).We find that, although to a lesser extent, even in images of complex objects, perception of material and illumination are basically confounded. Overall, illumination and material are confounded most when we present rendered spheres that differ only in reflectance mode under simple canonical illumination conditions. Interestingly, adding complex natural illumination containing higher order angular frequencies helps to disambiguate this confound in material judgments, but not in illumination judgments. Most helpful was the addition of 3D texture.


Perception | 2006

Material — Illumination Ambiguities and the Perception of Solid Objects

Sylvia C. Pont; Susan F. te Pas

The appearance of objects depends on their material, shape, and on the illumination conditions. Conversely, object appearance provides us with cues about the illumination and the material. This so-called inverse problem is basically underdetermined and therefore we expect that material and illumination perception are confounded. To gain insight into the relevant mechanisms, we rendered a set of artificial spheres for vastly different canonical light fields and reflectance functions. We used four physics-based bidirectional reflectance distribution functions (BRDFs) representing glossy, pitted, velvety, and matte material. The six illumination conditions were collimated illumination from four directions, hemispherical diffuse illumination, and fully diffuse (Ganzfeld) illumination. In three sub-experiments we presented pairs of stimuli and asked human observers to judge whether the material was the same, whether the illumination was the same, and for a subset in which either the illumination or the material was the same to judge which of the two was constant. We found that observers made many errors in all sub-experiments. In experiment 2 the illumination direction was chosen at random. Using an interactive interface, we asked human observers to match the illumination direction of a sphere of one of the four materials with that of a Lambertian sphere. We found systematical material-dependent deviations from veridical performance. Theoretical analysis of the radiance patterns suggests that judgments were based mainly on the position of the shadow edge. In conclusion, we found no evidence for ‘material constancy’ for perception of smooth rendered spheres despite vast quantitative and qualitative differences in illumination and in BRDF between the stimuli. Although human observers demonstrated some ‘illumination constancy’, they made systematic errors depending on the material reflectance, suggesting that they used mainly the location of the shadow edge. Our results suggest that material perception and light-field perception are basically confounded.


Journal of The Optical Society of America A-optics Image Science and Vision | 2003

Illumination direction from texture shading

Jan J. Koenderink; Andrea J. van Doorn; Astrid M. L. Kappers; Susan F. te Pas; Sylvia C. Pont

We investigate the ability of human observers to judge the direction of illumination from image texture. Photographs of 61 real surfaces were used, taken from the Columbia-Utrecht Reflectance and Texture (Curet) database (http:/www.cs.columbia.edu/CAVE/curet). All samples were normally viewed but obliquely illuminated, the elevation of the source being 22.5 degrees, 45.0 degrees, or 67.5 degrees. The illumination was with a collimated, parallel beam. Stimuli were presented in random orientation, and observers had to judge both the elevation and the azimuth of the source. Observers judged the azimuth within approximately 15 degrees, except for the fact that they committed random (with approximately 50% probability) sign flips (180 degrees flips). Connected with this finding is the fact that observers judged the illumination to be from above rather than below in the overwhelming majority of cases, despite the fact that each case occurred with equal probability. The elevation of the illumination can be judged to some extent but is not far above chance level. The data are in good agreement with a simple model that bases the estimate of illumination direction on the second-order statistics of local luminance gradients. This locates the locus of the probable mechanism very early in the visual stream.


Journal of Vision | 2005

Center-surround inhibition and facilitation as a function of size and contrast at multiple levels of visual motion processing

Chris L. E. Paffen; Maarten J. van der Smagt; Susan F. te Pas; Frans A. J. Verstraten

Visual context often plays a crucial role in visual processing. In the domain of visual motion processing, the response to a stimulus presented to a neurons classical receptive field can be modulated by presenting stimuli to its surround. The nature of these center-surround interactions is often inhibitory; the neural response decreases when the same direction of motion is presented to center and surround. Here we use binocular rivalry as a tool to study center-surround interactions. We show that magnitude of surround suppression varies as a function of luminance contrast and surround width. Increasing the size of surround motion increased surround suppression at high contrast. Furthermore, large, high-contrast surrounds facilitated opposite-direction motion in the center. For stimuli presented at low contrast, surround suppression peaked at a smaller surround width. In addition, we provide evidence that surround inhibition occurs at multiple levels of visual processing: Surround inhibition in motion processing is likely to originate from both monocular and binocular processing stages.


Vision Research | 2004

Center-surround interactions in visual motion processing during binocular rivalry

Chris L. E. Paffen; Susan F. te Pas; Ryota Kanai; Maarten J. van der Smagt; Frans A. J. Verstraten

When each eye is confronted with a dissimilar stimulus, the percept will generally alternate between the two. This phenomenon is known as binocular rivalry. Although binocular rivalry occurs at locations where targets overlap spatially, the area surrounding rivalrous targets can modulate their dominance. Here we show that during binocular rivalry of oppositely moving gratings, a surrounding grating moving in the same direction as one of the two leads to increased dominance of the opposite direction of motion in the center. This increased dominance of the opposite direction in the center was observed irrespective of the eye to which the surround was presented. Inspection of the results for different conditions reveals that the preference for the opposite direction of motion cannot be explained by a single mechanism operating beyond binocular fusion. We therefore suggest that this phenomenon is the outcome of center-surround interactions at multiple levels along the pathway of visual motion processing.


Perception | 1994

Haptic discrimination of doubly curved surfaces

Astrid M. L. Kappers; Jan J. Koenderink; Susan F. te Pas

Active haptic discrimination of mathematically well-defined surfaces was investigated. The quadric surfaces were defined in terms of ‘shape index’—a quantity describing shape—and ‘curvedness’—a quantity describing overall curvature. In these experiments shape index was varied and curvedness kept constant. The influence of laterality (unilateral versus bilateral discrimination) was tested in separate sessions. No influence of shape on the discrimination of solid objects was found. From the results an estimate can be made of the just-noticable difference in terms of shape-index units. There was a significant effect of laterality on discrimination: performance with unilateral (successive) examination (both with left and with right hand) was better than with bilateral (simultaneous) examination.


Vision Research | 1996

Detection of first-order structure in optic flow fields

Susan F. te Pas; Astrid M. L. Kappers; Jan J. Koenderink

We measured psychophysical thresholds for the detection of four different optic flow components in the presence of a translational velocity. We also measured thresholds for detection of rotation in the presence of expansion and for expansion in the presence of rotation. Our stimuli consisted of sparse random dot patterns. Detection thresholds are similar for all four optic flow components. Thus, our experiments indicate that our subjects use a factor that is similar in all first-order flow components, namely the relative orientation of the velocity vectors. First-order components can be extracted independently of both each other and translational velocity.


Behavioural Brain Research | 2018

Acute alerting effects of light: A systematic literature review

Jan L. Souman; Angelica Maria Tinga; Susan F. te Pas; Raymond van Ee; Björn N. S. Vlaskamp

HighlightsWe identified 68 publications that investigated acute alerting effects of light.Many studies reported increased subjective alertness with bright white light.No clear pattern of results for effects of CCT or wavelength was found.Better powered studies with larger sample sizes are needed. Abstract Periodic, well timed exposure to light is important for our health and wellbeing. Light, in particular in the blue part of the spectrum, is thought to affect alertness both indirectly, by modifying circadian rhythms, and directly, giving rise to acute effects. We performed a systematic review of empirical studies on direct, acute effects of light on alertness to evaluate the reliability of these effects. In total, we identified 68 studies in which either light intensity, spectral distribution, or both were manipulated, and evaluated the effects on behavioral measures of alertness, either subjectively or measured in reaction time performance tasks. The results show that increasing the intensity of polychromatic white light has been found to increase subjective ratings of alertness in a majority of studies, though a substantial proportion of studies failed to find significant effects, possibly due to small sample sizes or high baseline light intensities. The effect of the color temperature of white light on subjective alertness is less clear. Some studies found increased alertness with higher color temperatures, but other studies reported no detrimental effects of filtering out the short wavelengths from the spectrum. Similarly, studies that used monochromatic light exposure showed no systematic pattern for the effects of blue light compared to longer wavelengths. Far fewer studies investigated the effects of light intensity or spectrum on alertness as measured with reaction time tasks and of those, very few reported significant effects. In general, the small sample sizes used in studies on acute alerting effects of light make it difficult to draw definitive conclusions and better powered studies are needed, especially studies that allow for the construction of dose‐response curves.


Perception | 2004

Visual discrimination of spectral distributions

Susan F. te Pas; Jan J. Koenderink

Human observers seem to be able to use different features that classify materials with a large degree of accuracy. In this paper, we look at human perception of statistical properties of the spectral distribution in a scene. We investigated whether human observers can discriminate just as accurately between coloured textures that have a spectral distribution due either to shading only or to both shading and specular reflectance as between uniform colours. Thresholds for the discrimination of coloured textures are about 15 times as high as thresholds for the discrimination of uniform colours, provided there is a sharp transition between the two colours. However, the coloured texture thresholds are only 1.5 times higher when we introduce a gradual transition between the two colours. There are also distinct qualitative differences in discrimination thresholds for different base colours. These differences cannot be predicted from discrimination thresholds for uniform colours. Human observers are surprisingly good at discriminating between a material edge and a shadow edge in complex scenes. Statistical differences in the orientation of the colour distributions in colour space might be used to accomplish this. In a second experiment we investigated how well observers can discriminate between two linear distributions in colour space that have the same base colour but different orientations. When we vary the line-length in R, G, B space, thresholds cannot be predicted completely by the conservation of the average distance between the two distributions. This means that observers use not only the maximum colour difference in the stimulus to do the task, but other cues are also involved.

Collaboration


Dive into the Susan F. te Pas's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Sylvia C. Pont

Delft University of Technology

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jan J. Koenderink

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Huib de Ridder

Delft University of Technology

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Tatiana Kartashova

Delft University of Technology

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge