Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Susan Hepburn is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Susan Hepburn.


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry | 2003

Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders.

Sally J. Rogers; Susan Hepburn; Tracy Stackhouse; Elizabeth A. Wehner

BACKGROUND The present study sought to examine the specificity, developmental correlates, nature and pervasiveness of imitation deficits very early in the development of autism. METHODS Subjects were 24 children with autism (mean age 34 months), 18 children with fragile X syndrome, 20 children with other developmental disorders, and 15 typically-developing children. Tasks included manual, oral-facial, and object oriented imitations, developmental measures, joint attention ability, and motor abilities. RESULTS Children with autism were found to be significantly more impaired in overall imitation abilities, oral-facial imitation, and imitations of actions on objects than children in all of the other groups. Imitation skills of young children with fragile X syndrome were strongly influenced by the absence or presence of symptoms of autism. For children with autism, imitation skills were strongly correlated with autistic symptoms and joint attention, even when controlling for developmental level. For comparison groups, imitation was related to other developmental abilities including play, language, and visual spatial skills. Neither motor functioning nor social responsivity accounted for a significant amount of variance in imitation scores, when controlling for overall developmental level, which accounted for much of the variation in imitation ability. CONCLUSIONS Simple imitation skills were differentially impaired in young children with autism, and lack of social cooperation did not account for their poor performance. In autism, imitation skills clustered with dyadic and triadic social interactions and overall developmental level, but were not related to play or language development. For comparison children, all these areas were inter-related. Hypotheses about a specific dyspraxic deficit underlying the imitation performance in autism were not supported.


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry | 1999

Can Autism Be Diagnosed Accurately in Children Under 3 Years

Wendy L. Stone; Evon Batey Lee; Linda Ashford; Jane S. Brissie; Susan Hepburn; Elaine E. Coonrod; Bahr Weiss

This study investigated the reliability and stability of an autism diagnosis in children under 3 years of age who received independent diagnostic evaluations from two clinicians during two consecutive yearly evaluations. Strong evidence for the reliability and stability of the diagnosis was obtained. Diagnostic agreement between clinicians was higher for the broader discrimination of autism spectrum vs. no autism spectrum than for the more specific discrimination of autism vs. PDD-NOS. The diagnosis of autism at age 2 was more stable than the diagnosis of PDD-NOS at the same age. Social deficits and delays in spoken language were the most prominent DSM-IV characteristics evidenced by very young children with autism.


Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry | 2008

A replication of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) revised algorithms

Katherine Gotham; Susan Risi; Geraldine Dawson; Helen Tager-Flusberg; Robert M. Joseph; Alice S. Carter; Susan Hepburn; William M. McMahon; Patricia M. Rodier; Susan L. Hyman; Marian Sigman; Sally J. Rogers; Rebecca Landa; M. Anne Spence; Kathryn Osann; Pamela Flodman; Fred R. Volkmar; Eric Hollander; Joseph D. Buxbaum; Andrew Pickles; Catherine Lord

OBJECTIVE To replicate the factor structure and predictive validity of revised Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule algorithms in an independent dataset (N = 1,282). METHOD Algorithm revisions were replicated using data from children ages 18 months to 16 years collected at 11 North American sites participating in the Collaborative Programs for Excellence in Autism and the Studies to Advance Autism Research and Treatment. RESULTS Sensitivities and specificities approximated or exceeded those of the old algorithms except for young children with phrase speech and a clinical diagnosis of pervasive developmental disorders not otherwise specified. CONCLUSIONS Revised algorithms increase comparability between modules and improve the predictive validity of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule for autism cases compared to the original algorithms.


Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | 1997

Nonverbal Communication in Two- and Three-Year-Old Children with Autism.

Wendy L. Stone; Opal Y. Ousley; Paul J. Yoder; Kerry L. Hogan; Susan Hepburn

The forms, functions, and complexity of nonverbal communication used by very young children with autism were investigated. Fourteen children with autism were matched to 14 children with developmental delays and/or language impairments on the basis of CA, MA, and expressive vocabulary. Subjects participated in a structured communication assessment consisting of 16 situations designed to elicit requesting or commenting behavior. Children with autism requested more often and commented less often than controls. Autistic children were less likely to point, show objects, or use eye gaze to communicate, but were more likely to directly manipulate the examiners hand. The autistic group also used less complex combinations of behaviors to communicate. Implications for early identification and intervention are discussed.


American Journal on Mental Retardation | 2004

Linguistic and Cognitive Functioning and Autism Symptoms in Young Children With Fragile X syndrome

Amy Philofsky; Susan Hepburn; Athena Hayes; Randi J. Hagerman; Sally J. Rogers

Linguistic and cognitive profiles were examined in 18 children with autism and 18 children with fragile X syndrome (mean ages = 34 months). State-of-the-art diagnostic procedures for autism symptom identification were administered. Eight children with fragile X met criteria for autism. Comparison of linguistic and cognitive profiles (autism, fragile X without autism, fragile X with autism) revealed that children with fragile X (with autism) were more impaired in nonverbal cognition and expressive language. Receptive language was a relative strength for children with fragile X (without autism). There were no differences in receptive language in children with autism, regardless of fragile X status. Low receptive language may be a marker for autism symptoms in young children with fragile X.


Developmental Neuropsychology | 2005

Early Regression in Social Communication in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A CPEA Study

Rhiannon J. Luyster; Jennifer Richler; Susan Risi; Wan Ling Hsu; Geraldine Dawson; Raphael Bernier; Michelle Dunn; Susan Hepburn; Susan L. Hyman; William M. McMahon; Julie Goudie-Nice; Nancy J. Minshew; Sally J. Rogers; Marian Sigman; M. Anne Spence; Wendy A. Goldberg; Helen Tager-Flusberg; Fred R. Volkmar; Catherine Lord

In a multisite study of 351 children with autism spectrum disorders, 21 children with developmental delays, and 31 children with typical development, this study used caregiver interviews (i.e., the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised) at the time of entry into other research projects and follow-up telephone interviews designed for this project to describe the childrens early acquisition and loss of social-communication milestones. Children who had used words spontaneously and meaningfully and then stopped talking were described by their caregivers as showing more gestures, greater participation in social games, and better receptive language before the loss and fewer of these skills after the loss than other children with autism spectrum disorders. A significant minority of children with autism without word loss showed a very similar pattern of loss of social-communication skills, a pattern not observed in the children with developmental delays or typical development.


Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities | 2009

Cognitive-Behavioral Group Treatment for Anxiety Symptoms in Children With High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders A Pilot Study

Judith A. Reaven; Audrey Blakeley-Smith; Shana Nichols; Meena Dasari; Erin Flanigan; Susan Hepburn

Individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are at increased risk for developing anxiety disorders relative to children without ASD and those with other developmental disabilities. Thirty-three children with high-functioning ASD and their parents participated in an original, manualized cognitive behavioral group treatment aimed at reducing severity of anxiety symptoms. Parent-child dyads entered into either an Active Treatment Condition or Wait List Control Condition. Results indicated significant reductions in parent report of anxiety symptoms after the delivery of the group treatment, compared with the Wait List Control Condition. The findings of this study are promising, particularly in light of the high rates of comorbidity between ASD and anxiety. Limitations and recommendations for future research directions are discussed.


Autism | 2003

Cognitive-Behavioral Treatment of Obsessive- Compulsive Disorder in a Child with Asperger Syndrome A Case Report

Judy Reaven; Susan Hepburn

This case report outlines the cognitive-behavioral treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder in a 7-year-old female with Asperger syndrome. Interventions were based upon the work of March and Mulle and were adapted in light of the patient’s cognitive, social, and linguistic characteristics. Obsessive-compulsive symptoms improved markedly after approximately 6 months of treatment. Issues regarding symptom presentation, assessment, and treatment of a dually diagnosed patient are discussed.


American Journal on Mental Retardation | 1999

Patterns of Adaptive Behavior in Very Young Children with Autism.

Wendy L. Stone; Opal Ousley; Susan Hepburn; Kerry L. Hogan; Christia Spears Brown

The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales were used to investigate patterns of adaptive behavior in children with autism who were under 36 months of age. Subjects were 30 children with autism and 30 children with developmental delay matched on CA and MA. Relative to controls, the autistic group demonstrated weaker socialization and communication skills and greater discrepancies between adaptive behavior and MA. Different patterns of relations between adaptive behavior domains and cognitive and language skills were obtained for the two groups. Preliminary support for the utility of adaptive behavior profiles in identifying subgroups of children with autism is provided. Results are discussed in terms of their implications for early diagnosis of autism.


NeuroImage | 2014

Decreased left perisylvian GABA concentration in children with autism and unaffected siblings

Donald C. Rojas; Debra Singel; Sarah Steinmetz; Susan Hepburn; Mark S. Brown

Imbalanced levels of excitation and inhibition (E/I) have been proposed to account for various behavioral and electrophysiological phenotypes in autism. Although proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy ((1)H-MRS) studies have been published on various metabolite levels in autism, including glutamate, the major excitatory neurotransmitter, few (1)H-MRS studies have yet been conducted the major inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA. Seventeen individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) participated in a single-voxel, point resolved spectroscopy (PRESS) study conducted on a 3T magnet. Data were also acquired on 14 unaffected siblings of children with autism, and 17 age- and gender-matched healthy control subjects. GABA concentration was measured along with Creatine (Cr) in a single voxel aligned with the auditory cortex in the perisylvian region of the left hemisphere. The ratio of GABA to Cr was significantly lower in the ASD group than the control subjects. Siblings also exhibited lower GABA/Cr ratios compared to controls. Cr concentration did not differ between groups. The volumes of gray matter, white matter and CSF did not differ between groups in the whole brain or within the spectroscopy voxel. Reduced auditory GABA concentration in ASD is consistent with one previous MRS study of GABA concentration in the frontal lobe in autism, suggesting that multiple neocortical areas may be involved. Lower GABA levels are consistent with theories of ASD as a disorder involving impaired inhibitory neurotransmission and E/I imbalance. The reduction in unaffected siblings suggests that it may be a heritable biomarker, or endophenotype, of autism.

Collaboration


Dive into the Susan Hepburn's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Audrey Blakeley-Smith

University of Colorado Denver

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Donald C. Rojas

University of Colorado Denver

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Judy Reaven

Anschutz Medical Campus

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Amy Philofsky

University of Colorado Denver

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Cordelia Robinson

University of Colorado Denver

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Erin Winterrowd

University of Wisconsin–Oshkosh

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge