Tenji Wake
Chukyo University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Tenji Wake.
Perceptual and Motor Skills | 1982
Yutaka Shimizu; Tenji Wake
Tactile sensitivity to shifts of a point stimulus was determined at the forehead for spatially filled (continuous) and spatially separated (discrete) stimulation. The minimal limen was between 12 and 30 mm/sec. for continuous stimulation and at a 200-msec. interstimulus interval for discrete stimulation. The continuous thresholds were always lower than the discrete thresholds at the examined time rates. Furthermore, the continuous thresholds were slightly higher for oblique directions than for both vertical and horizontal directions, while the discrete thresholds were quite low for both vertical and horizontal directions.
Perceptual and Motor Skills | 2012
Yuki Miyazaki; Hiromi Wake; Shigeru Ichihara; Tenji Wake
Previous research has suggested that a singly presented facial stimulus having a direct gaze holds spatial attention. This study examined whether facial stimulus having a direct gaze can also capture spatial attention in a relative dot-probe paradigm (facial stimulus having a direct gaze was presented concurrently with that having an averted gaze). The results showed that participants oriented their spatial attention to a facial stimulus having a direct gaze rather than to that with an averted gaze. This attentional bias depended on gaze-perception mechanisms as observed in the lack of attentional bias to a direct gaze from unnatural-looking eyes (i.e., white pupil/iris and black sclera). These findings raise the possibility that the attentional effect implicated in the perception of a direct gaze is induced regardless of the stimulus context.
Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 2015
Takako Yoshida; Ayumi Yamaguchi; Hideomi Tsutsui; Tenji Wake
Haptic perception of a 2D image is thought to make heavy demands on working memory. During active exploration, humans need to store the latest local sensory information and integrate it with kinesthetic information from hand and finger locations in order to generate a coherent perception. This tactile integration has not been studied as extensively as visual shape integration. In the current study, we compared working-memory capacity for tactile exploration to that of visual exploration as measured in change-detection tasks. We found smaller memory capacity during tactile exploration (approximately 1 item) compared with visual exploration (2–10 items). These differences generalized to position memory and could not be attributed to insufficient stimulus-exposure durations, acuity differences between modalities, or uncertainty over the position of items. This low capacity for tactile memory suggests that the haptic system is almost amnesic when outside the fingertips and that there is little or no cross-position integration.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1996
Tenji Wake; Hiromi Wake
Tactile apparent movement was observed under the conditions of variation in stimulus duration, frequency, and separation between two stimuli. First, optimal stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) was determined as a function of stimulus duration using stimuli with the same frequency. Second, optimal SOA was estimated as a function of frequency, by using two stimuli with different frequencies. The optimal SOA for tactile apparent movement increased with the increases of stimulus duration. When two vibrators activate with the same frequency, the optimal SOA depends on frequency. The optimal SOA of the low‐frequency stimulus (10 Hz) is longer than that of the high‐frequency stimulus (250 Hz). If the first stimulus is 10 Hz and the second stimulus is 200 Hz, the optimal SOA is shorter than two stimuli with same frequency (10 Hz). The first stimulus was kept constant at 10 Hz and the second stimulus was varied in frequency. As the frequency‐difference increases, the optimal SOA is decreased, but at the second stimulu...
Perception | 2014
Hiromi Wake; Tenji Wake; Tadasu Oyama
We present a novel three-dimensional (3-D) version of Rubins classical bistable goblet–profiles figure. An actual goblet sculpture was produced and rotated on a turntable in front of a white background. As the goblet rotates about its central axis, small circular asymmetries around the lips and chin region give a clear impression of two white profiles talking to each other. Although the profiles actually correspond to empty space or white background, they are more likely to be perceived as ‘figure’ than the 3-D goblet itself. Four experiments that presented the actual goblet (experiment 1) or two-dimensional (2-D) movies of it (experiments 2–4) were designed to verify these observations. We measured perceptual dominance of profiles as ‘figure’ and rate of reversal as a function of three factors: Motion (static vs rotating), orientation (upright vs inverted), and configuration (face-to-face vs back-to-back). Results for the rotating goblet showed a statistically reliable preference for perceiving the talking profiles as ‘figure’. Deforming the profiles by manipulating the vertex angle of the mouth region produced an inverted U-shaped curve with the peak representing the stimulus condition in which the profiles perception was most remarkable. We discussed a number of 3-D and 2-D figure-ground factors that might apply to this rather complex stimulus situation.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1996
Hiromi Wake; Tenji Wake
The so‐called subjective‐contour effect was examined in touch and compared with vision. The first experiment was designed to establish a criterion of the subjective pattern by modal completion. In experiment 2, free verbal reports were gathered from naive subjects. Verbal reports and rating scores for three characteristics, subjective pattern, depth displacement, and brightness transformation, were gathered from the well‐trained subjects in experiment 3. Results were as follows. (1) The effect was strong in vision especially for the pattern. Subjective pattern was reported in touch, but, in most other cases, they understood the pattern to be just a triangle or square as a result of bridging, without perceiving the contiguous surface. (2) Subjective depth was not observed so much even in vision by the naive subjects, although it was clear to the well‐trained subjects. When depth displacement was reported, the subjective surface was perceived as more concave than the surface of paper. In conclusion, althoug...
Journal of The Illuminating Engineering Institute of Japan | 1994
Chiaki Tonami; Naoyoshi Nameda; Tenji Wake
This paper reports the seeing properties for traffic signs by blurred vision, When a deteriorated eye of an aged person, a non corrected near-sighted eye, or peripheral vision is used to observe traffic signs, this investigation is to discover what spatial frequency components of the sign pattern is effective to recognition. There are many traffic signs, such as limit of car speed, warnings, and so on. For this study, 18 signs were chosen from 46 signs. Chosen sign patterns were representative of the typically recognized patterns. Every patterns were previously blurred by Macintosh computer to simulate blurred vision. The patterns were then displayed for a sufficient time to 10 subjects. 9 students were all 20 years of age and 1 aged person was 50 years of age. None of these subjects had any special defect to their eyes. Students were all corrected by glasses, if they were near sighted. An aged subject had no glasses. For speculation of the sign patterns features, each blurred pattern was dissolved by Fourier Transformation by the computer.
Technology and Disability | 1999
Hiromi Wake; Tenji Wake; Hiroshi Takahashi
Japanese Journal of Psychology | 2003
Takako Yoshida; Tenji Wake; Naoyuki Osaka
Journal of the Color Science Association of Japan | 2013
Ken-ichiro Kawamoto; Tenji Wake; Hiromi Wake