Thomas E. Malloy
Rhode Island College
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Thomas E. Malloy.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 1988
Linda Albright; David A. Kenny; Thomas E. Malloy
This research focused on the target effect on a perceivers judgments of personality when the perceiver and the target are unacquainted. The perceiver was given no opportunity to interact with the target, a condition we refer to as zero acquaintance. We reasoned that in order to make personality judgments, perceivers would use the information available to them (physical appearance). Consensus in personality judgments would result, then, from shared stereotypes about particular physical appearance characteristics. Results from three separate studies with 259 subjects supported this hypothesis. On two of the five dimensions (extraversion and conscientiousness) on which subjects rated each other, a significant proportion of variance was due to the stimulus target. Consensus on judgments of extraversion appears to have been largely mediated by judgments of physical attractiveness. Across the three studies there was also evidence that the consensus in judgments on these two dimensions had some validity, in that they correlated with self-judgments on those two dimensions.
Health Psychology | 1996
Jeffrey D. Fisher; William A. Fisher; Stephen J. Misovich; Diane L. Kimble; Thomas E. Malloy
This research used the Information-Motivation-Behavioral Skills (IMB) model of AIDS risk behavior change (J. D. Fisher & Fisher, 1992a) to reduce AIDS risk behavior in a college student population. College students received an IMB model-based intervention that addressed AIDS risk reduction information, motivation, and behavioral skills deficits that had been empirically identified in this population, or were assigned to a no-treatment control condition. At a 1-month follow-up, results confirmed that the intervention resulted in increases in AIDS risk reduction information, motivation, and behavioral skills, as well as significant increases in condom accessibility, safer sex negotiations, and condom use during sexual intercourse. At a long-term follow-up, the intervention again resulted in significant increases in AIDS preventive behaviors.
Psychological Bulletin | 1994
David A. Kenny; Linda Albright; Thomas E. Malloy; Deborah A. Kashy
Consensus refers to the extent to which judges agree in their ratings of a common target. Consensus has been an important area of research in social and personality psychology. In this article, generalizability theory is used to develop a percentage of total variance measure of consensus. This measure is used to review the level of consensus across 32 studies by considering the role of acquaintance level and trait dimension. The review indicates that consensus correlations ranged from zero to about .3, with higher levels of consensus for ratings of Extraversion. The studies do not provide evidence that consensus increases with increasing acquaintance, a counterintuitive result that can be accounted for by a theoretical model (D.A. Kenny, 1991, in press). Problems in the interpretation of longitudinal research are reviewed.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 1990
Thomas E. Malloy; Linda Albright
Interpersonal perception among well-acquainted individuals in a social context was studied. High acquaintance was expected to provide perceivers with a large sample of target behaviors across situations. In turn, memory for acquaintances should be organized by social group and personality characteristics, as predicted by the social context-personality index theory. Differentiation of the targets traits in memory should produce a target effect on perception that is stronger than the perceiver effect. Furthermore, evidence for accuracy, meta-accuracy, independence of self- and other-perception, and reciprocity of affect were anticipated. A social relations analysis of data from a multiple-interaction, reciprocal design was used to study these phenomena. At the individual level, analyses indicated that perceptions of targets were determined primarily by target characteristics and secondarily by perceiver construction of the judgment. Also, perceivers judged targets as targets judged themselves, and targets knew in general how perceivers viewed them. Self- and other-perceptions were largely independent. Surprisingly, we did not observe dyadic meta-accuracy.
Aids and Behavior | 1999
William A. Fisher; Sunyna S. Williams; Jeffrey D. Fisher; Thomas E. Malloy
The current research applied the information-motivation-behavioral skills (IMB) model to examine psychological determinants of AIDS risk behavior in a sample of sexually active, primarily minority, urban adolescents (N = 148) drawn from an area of high adolescent HIV seroprevalence. Structural equation modeling demonstrated that the IMB model fit the data and that the constructs of the model accounted for 46–75% of the variance in AIDS risk behavior among sexually active urban male and female adolescents. Structural equation modeling also examined the fit of a restricted special case of the IMB model, focusing on motivation and behavioral skills as determinants of AIDS risk behavior, and showed that this model fit the data as well. Discussion focuses on the generality of the IMB model of AIDS risk behavior across diverse populations at risk, on the comparative value of the full IMB model and the restricted special case of this model, and on the implications of these findings for interventions to reduce AIDS risk behavior among sexually active urban adolescents.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 1997
Thomas E. Malloy; Linda Albright; David A. Kenny; Fredric Agatstein; Lynn Winquist
Consensus, self-other agreement, and meta-accuracy were studied within and across nonoverlapping social groups. Thirty-one target persons were judged on the Big Five factors by 9 informants: 3 family members, 3 friends, and 3 coworkers. Although well acquainted within groups, informants were unacquainted between groups. A social relations analysis conducted within each social group showed reliable consensus on the Big Five personality factors. A model specified to estimate the consistency of a target persons effect on perceptions by others across social groups showed weaker agreement across groups. That is, targets were perceived consensually within groups, but these consensual perceptions differed between groups. The data suggest that personality and identity are context specific; however, there was some evidence of agreement in perceptions across groups.
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior | 1988
David A. Kenny; Thomas E. Malloy
A persons behavior during social interaction is due not only to the persons dispositional characteristics but is also determined by his or her social partner. If a person consistently elicits the same behavior from others, the person has apartner effect. Partner effects in affect, cognition, and behavior are examined. Partner effects in behavior are presumed to be caused by partner effects in affect and cognition. A social relations analysis of data from six studies which meet rather strict methodological criteria suggests that partner effects are present in affect and cognition but are fairly weak. Further, partner effects are hardly evident at all on behavioral observations of friendliness and dominance, but are more evident in nonverbal behavior, especially gaze. The low level of partner effects is hypothesized to be due to the context in which the research was done, the degree of acquaintance between interaction partners, and the moderating effect of individual differences.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin | 2004
Thomas E. Malloy; Linda Albright; Rolando Diaz-Loving; Qi Dong; Yueh-Ting Lee
The social context hypothesis states that people behave differently in different social groups because group norms and context-specific interpersonal relationships uniquely affect behavior. Consequently, a person who is a member of different, nonoverlapping social groups (i. e., the members of different groups are unacquainted) should be judged consensually on personality traits within each group; however, between groups there should be less agreement in judgments. This research focused on cultural moderation of the social context effect in two collective cultures (China and Mexico) with different norms for interpersonal relationships. Among Chinese, there was greater consensus in trait judgments within groups than between groups, whereas in Mexico, agreement within and between groups was equivalent. Culturally based relationship norms that affect cross-context consistency of behavior and, in turn, the consistency of trait judgments across groups were described.
Personality and Social Psychology Review | 2006
David A. Kenny; Tessa V. West; Thomas E. Malloy; Linda Albright
We examine the advantages and disadvantages of 2 types of analyses used in interpersonal perception studies: componential and noncomponential. Componential analysis of interpersonal perception data (Kenny, 1994) partitions a judgment into components and then estimates the variances of and the correlations between these components. A noncomponential analysis uses raw scores to analyze interpersonal perception data. Three different research areas are investigated: consensus of perceptions across social contexts, reciprocity of attraction, and individual differences in self-enhancement. Finally, we consider criticisms of componential analysis. We conclude that interpersonal perception data necessarily have components (e.g., perceiver, target, measure, and their interactions), and that the researcher needs to develop a model that best captures the researchers questions.
Basic and Applied Social Psychology | 2004
Stephen Peters; Penelope Kinsey; Thomas E. Malloy
Guided by Eaglys (1987) social role theory and Fiskes (1993) model of status effects in dyads, we studied leadership perceptions within and between gender among African American college students engaged in a problem solving task. Variance components in leadership judgments replicated the pattern of findings reported by Malloy and Janowski (1992), and extended them by showing more consensus in leadership judgments of men by both men and women. Furthermore, there was more consensus in womens judgments of men than in mens judgments of women, and men more accurately predicted how they were judged by women than by men. Reliable self-other agreement of leadership judgments was only observed for men (self) when judged by women. Results revealed a gender bias in leadership perceptions among African Americans that is similar to that observed among European Americans, and were more consistent with a gender inequality model than with a gender equality model.