Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Torbjörn Åkerstedt is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Torbjörn Åkerstedt.


Chronobiology International | 2008

Sleepiness and performance in response to repeated sleep restriction and subsequent recovery during semi-laboratory conditions.

John Axelsson; Göran Kecklund; Torbjörn Åkerstedt; Paolo D'Onofrio; Mats Lekander; Michael Ingre

There is an ongoing debate of how best to measure the effects of sleep loss in a reliable and feasible way, partly because well controlled laboratory studies and field studies have come to different conclusions. The aims of the present study were to investigate both sleepiness and performance in response to long‐term sleep restriction and recovery in a semi‐laboratory environment, investigate order effects (i.e., whether levels return to baseline) in a study with seven days of recovery, and characterize individual differences in tolerance to restricted sleep. Nine healthy men (age 23–28 yrs) participated in the protocol, which included one habituation day (sleep 23:00–07:00 h), two baseline days (23:00–07:00 h), five days with restricted sleep (03:00–07:00 h), and seven recovery days (23:00–07:00 h). Participants went outdoors at least twice each day. Reaction‐time tests were performed at 08:00, 14:00, and 20:00 h each day in the laboratory. Sleepiness was self‐rated by the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) after each test. The mixed‐effect regression models showed that each day of restricted sleep resulted in an increase of sleepiness by 0.64±.05 KSS units (a nine‐step scale, p<.001), increase of median reaction times of 6.6±1.6 ms (p=.003), and increase of lapses/test of 0.69±.16 ms (p<.001). Seven days of recovery allowed participants to return to the baseline for sleepiness and median reaction time, but not for lapses. The individual differences were larger for performance measures than for sleepiness; the between‐subject standard deviation for the random intercept was in the magnitude of the effects of 1.1 days of restricted sleep for sleepiness, 6.6 days of restricted sleep for median reaction time, and 3.2 days for lapses. In conclusion, the present study shows that sleepiness is closely related to sleep pressure, while performance measures, to a larger extent, appear determined by specific individual traits. Moreover, it is suggested to measure sleepiness in a standardized situation so as to minimize the influences of contextual factors.


Journal of Sleep Research | 2009

Reaction of sleepiness indicators to partial sleep deprivation, time of day and time on task in a driving simulator : the DROWSI project

Torbjörn Åkerstedt; Michael Ingre; Göran Kecklund; Anna Anund; David Sandberg; Mattias Wahde; Pierre Philip; Peter Kronberg

Studies of driving and sleepiness indicators have mainly focused on prior sleep reduction. The present study sought to identify sleepiness indicators responsive to several potential regulators of sleepiness: sleep loss, time of day (TOD) and time on task (TOT) during simulator driving. Thirteen subjects drove a high‐fidelity moving base simulator in six 1‐h sessions across a 24‐h period, after normal sleep duration (8 h) and after partial sleep deprivation (PSD; 4 h). The results showed clear main effects of TOD (night) and TOT but not for PSD, although the latter strongly interacted with TOD. The most sensitive variable was subjective sleepiness, the standard deviation of lateral position (SDLAT) and measures of eye closure [duration, speed (slow), amplitude (low)]. Measures of electroencephalography and line crossings (LCs) showed only modest responses. For most variables individual differences vastly exceeded those of the fixed effects, except for subjective sleepiness and SDLAT. In a multiple regression analysis, SDLAT, amplitude/peak eye‐lid closing velocity and blink duration predicted subjective sleepiness bouts with a sensitivity and specificity of about 70%, but were mutually redundant. The prediction of LCs gave considerably weaker, but similar results. In summary, SDLAT and eye closure variables could be candidates for use in sleepiness‐monitoring devices. However, individual differences are considerable and there is need for research on how to identify and predict individual differences in susceptibility to sleepiness.


Acta Paediatrica | 2009

Playing a violent television game affects heart rate variability

Malena Ivarsson; Michael Anderson; Torbjörn Åkerstedt; Frank Lindblad

Objective: To investigate how playing a violent/nonviolent television game during the evening affects sympathetic and parasympathetic reactions during and after playing as well as sleep quality during the night after playing.


Chronobiology International | 2008

Accounting for Partial Sleep Deprivation and Cumulative Sleepiness in the Three‐Process Model of Alertness Regulation

Torbjörn Åkerstedt; Michael Ingre; Göran Kecklund; Simon Folkard; John Axelsson

Mathematical models designed to predict alertness or performance have been developed primarily as tools for evaluating work and/or sleep‐wake schedules that deviate from the traditional daytime orientation. In general, these models cope well with the acute changes resulting from an abnormal sleep but have difficulties handling sleep restriction across longer periods. The reason is that the function representing recovery is too steep—usually exponentially so—and with increasing sleep loss, the steepness increases, resulting in too rapid recovery. The present study focused on refining the Three‐Process Model of alertness regulation. We used an experiment with 4 h of sleep/night (nine participants) that included subjective self‐ratings of sleepiness every hour. To evaluate the model at the individual subject level, a set of mixed‐effect regression analyses were performed using subjective sleepiness as the dependent variable. These mixed models estimate a fixed effect (group mean) and a random effect that accounts for heterogeneity between participants in the overall level of sleepiness (i.e., a random intercept). Using this technique, a point was sought on the exponential recovery function that would explain maximum variance in subjective sleepiness by switching to a linear function. The resulting point explaining the highest amount of variance was 12.2 on the 1–21 unit scale. It was concluded that the accumulation of sleep loss effects on subjective sleepiness may be accounted for by making the recovery function linear below a certain point on the otherwise exponential function.


Ergonomics | 1977

Field studies of shiftwork: I. Temporal patterns in psychophysiological activation in permanent night workers.

P. Patkai; Torbjörn Åkerstedt; K. Pettersson

Abstract The diurnal variation in body temperature, catecholamine excretion, performance, and subjective alertness was studied among 24 permanent night workers during the 1st, 3rd, and 5th day of their work ‘ week ’of six nights. Most of the variables showed a significant diurnal pattern with high values during work. Furthermore, the patterns of variation were similar during the three nights of measurement without any observable adjustment over the week. It was therefore suggested that permanent night work might facilitate a more rapid switch over to night work after free days.


Chronobiology International | 2010

SLEEP AND SLEEPINESS: IMPACT OF ENTERING OR LEAVING SHIFTWORK—A PROSPECTIVE STUDY

Torbjörn Åkerstedt; Maria Nordin; Lars Alfredsson; Peter Westerholm; Göran Kecklund

Very little is known about the effects on sleep and sleepiness of entering or exiting shiftwork. The present study used a longitudinal database (n = 3637). Participants completed a questionnaire on work hours, sleep, and work environment at the start and end of a 5-yr period. Changes in shift/day work status were related to change in a number of subjective sleep variables using logistic regression analysis. The analyses were adjusted for age, sex, and differences in socioeconomic status, work demands, work control, physical workload, marriage status, and number of children. In comparison with constant day work, entering shiftwork (with or without night shifts) from day work increased the risk of difficulties in falling asleep, and leaving shiftwork reduced this risk (odds ratio [OR] = 2.8 [confidence interval, CI = 1.8–4.5]). Also falling asleep at work showed a consistent pattern; an increased risk of falling asleep for those with shiftwork on both occasions, and for those with night work on both occasions. Also entering night work was associated with a strongly increased risk of falling asleep at work (OR = 2.9 [CI = 1.3–6.7]). These results suggest that entering and leaving shiftwork has a considerable impact on sleep and alertness. However, there is a need for large and more extended longitudinal studies to support our findings. (Author correspondence: [email protected])


Chronobiology International | 2008

Sleep Length as a Function of Morning Shift‐Start Time in Irregular Shift Schedules for Train Drivers: Self‐Rated Health and Individual Differences

Michael Ingre; Göran Kecklund; Torbjörn Åkerstedt; Marie Söderström; Lena Kecklund

Forty‐six male train drivers (mean age=46.5, SD=5.1) were recruited to participate in a diary study for 14 consecutive days with questions about their sleep and working hours. A polynomial mixed‐effect regression model showed a curvilinear relation (p <. 001) between shift‐start time and sleep duration for shifts starting at 03∶00–12∶00 h and with a near linear increase for ones starting between 04∶30 and 09∶00 h of approximately 0.7 h for every 1 h the shift was delayed. The longest sleeps were estimated at ∼8 h before shifts that started at ∼10∶00 h. The shortest sleeps were found for shifts that started before 04∶30 h and were estimated at ∼5 h. Individual differences were estimated with a random-effect standard deviation of 0.51 h, independent of shift‐start time (p =.005). One‐half of the between‐subject variance was explained by subjective health. A one‐step decrease in health was associated with a 26 min increase in sleep length. The results have practical implications for constructing shift schedules. Early morning shifts reduced sleep length substantially and should be mixed with later start hours to avoid the accumulation of sleep dept. Delaying the shift‐start past 10∶00 h had little effect on sleep opportunity; however, delaying shift‐start to between 04∶30 and 9∶00 h had a strong impact on sleep length, with 70% of the extra time used for sleep, suggesting large positive effects for this range of shift‐start times.


Scandinavian Journal of Public Health | 2012

Psychosocial stress and health problems: Health in Sweden: The National Public Health Report 2012. Chapter 6.

Maria Danielsson; Inger Heimerson; Ulf Lundberg; Aleksander Perski; Claes-Göran Stefansson; Torbjörn Åkerstedt

Stress can be defined as an imbalance between demands placed on us and our ability to manage them. The body’s stress system is adapted to confront sudden physical threats. Today, however, we are in ...


Occupational and Environmental Medicine | 2013

Shift-related sleep problems vary according to work schedule

Elisabeth Flo; Ståle Pallesen; Torbjörn Åkerstedt; Nils Magerøy; Bente E. Moen; Janne Grønli; Inger Hilde Nordhus

Objectives Shift-related sleep and sleepiness problems may be due to characteristics of both shifts (ie, day, evening and night shifts) and work schedules (ie, permanent vs rotational schedules). The Bergen Shift Work Sleep Questionnaire (BSWSQ) was used to investigate associations between shift-related sleep problems and work schedules. Methods 1586 nurses completed the BSWSQ. Participants who, in relation to a shift, ‘often’ or ‘always’ experienced both a sleep problem and a tiredness/sleepiness problem were defined as having shift-related insomnia (separate for day, evening and night shifts and rest-days). Logistic regression analyses were conducted for day, evening, night, and rest-day insomnia with participants on both permanent and rotational schedules. Results Shift-related insomnia differed between the work schedules. The evening shift insomnia was more prevalent in the two-shift rotation schedule than the three-shift rotation schedule (29.8% and 19.8%, respectively). Night shift insomnia showed higher frequencies among three-shift rotation workers compared with permanent night workers (67.7% and 41.7%, respectively). Rest-day insomnia was more prevalent among permanent night workers compared with two- and three-shift rotations (11.4% compared with 4.2% and 3.6%, respectively). Conclusions The prevalences of shift-related insomnia differed between the work schedules with higher frequencies for three-shift rotations and night shifts. However, sleep problems were present in all shifts and schedules. This suggests that both shifts and work schedules should be considered in the study of shift work-related sleep problems.


Journal of Sleep Research | 2012

In-car countermeasures open window and music revisited on the real road: popular but hardly effective against driver sleepiness

Johanna Schwarz; Michael Ingre; Carina Fors; Anna Anund; Göran Kecklund; Jacques Taillard; Pierre Philip; Torbjörn Åkerstedt

This study investigated the effects of two very commonly used countermeasures against driver sleepiness, opening the window and listening to music, on subjective and physiological sleepiness measures during real road driving. In total, 24 individuals participated in the study. Sixteen participants received intermittent 10‐min intervals of: (i) open window (2 cm opened); and (ii) listening to music, during both day and night driving on an open motorway. Both subjective sleepiness and physiological sleepiness (blink duration) was estimated to be significantly reduced when subjects listened to music, but the effect was only minor compared with the pronounced effects of night driving and driving duration. Open window had no attenuating effect on either sleepiness measure. No significant long‐term effects beyond the actual countermeasure application intervals occurred, as shown by comparison to the control group (n = 8). Thus, despite their popularity, opening the window and listening to music cannot be recommended as sole countermeasures against driver sleepiness.

Collaboration


Dive into the Torbjörn Åkerstedt's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Rolf Ekman

University of Gothenburg

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Gregory Belenky

Washington State University Spokane

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge