Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where William Hoppitt is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by William Hoppitt.


Learning & Behavior | 2016

How New Caledonian crows solve novel foraging problems and what it means for cumulative culture

Corina Jill Logan; Alexis J. Breen; Alex H. Taylor; Russell D. Gray; William Hoppitt

New Caledonian crows make and use tools, and tool types vary over geographic landscapes. Social learning may explain the variation in tool design, but it is unknown to what degree social learning accounts for the maintenance of these designs. Indeed, little is known about the mechanisms these crows use to obtain information from others, despite the question’s importance in understanding whether tool behavior is transmitted via social, genetic, or environmental means. For social transmission to account for tool-type variation, copying must utilize a mechanism that is action specific (e.g., pushing left vs. right) as well as context specific (e.g., pushing a particular object vs. any object). To determine whether crows can copy a demonstrator’s actions as well as the contexts in which they occur, we conducted a diffusion experiment using a novel foraging task. We used a nontool task to eliminate any confounds introduced by individual differences in their prior tool experience. Two groups had demonstrators (trained in isolation on different options of a four-option task, including a two-action option) and one group did not. We found that crows socially learn about context: After observers see a demonstrator interact with the task, they are more likely to interact with the same parts of the task. In contrast, observers did not copy the demonstrator’s specific actions. Our results suggest it is unlikely that observing tool-making behavior transmits tool types. We suggest it is possible that tool types are transmitted when crows copy the physical form of the tools they encounter.


Royal Society Open Science | 2016

Social networks predict selective observation and information spread in ravens

Ipek G. Kulahci; Daniel I. Rubenstein; Thomas Bugnyar; William Hoppitt; Nace Mikus; Christine Schwab

Animals are predicted to selectively observe and learn from the conspecifics with whom they share social connections. Yet, hardly anything is known about the role of different connections in observation and learning. To address the relationships between social connections, observation and learning, we investigated transmission of information in two raven (Corvus corax) groups. First, we quantified social connections in each group by constructing networks on affiliative interactions, aggressive interactions and proximity. We then seeded novel information by training one group member on a novel task and allowing others to observe. In each group, an observation network based on who observed whose task-solving behaviour was strongly correlated with networks based on affiliative interactions and proximity. Ravens with high social centrality (strength, eigenvector, information centrality) in the affiliative interaction network were also central in the observation network, possibly as a result of solving the task sooner. Network-based diffusion analysis revealed that the order that ravens first solved the task was best predicted by connections in the affiliative interaction network in a group of subadult ravens, and by social rank and kinship (which influenced affiliative interactions) in a group of juvenile ravens. Our results demonstrate that not all social connections are equally effective at predicting the patterns of selective observation and information transmission.


Royal Society Open Science | 2017

Social learning in otters

Zosia Ladds; William Hoppitt; Neeltje J. Boogert

The use of information provided by others to tackle lifes challenges is widespread, but should not be employed indiscriminately if it is to be adaptive. Evidence is accumulating that animals are indeed selective and adopt ‘social learning strategies’. However, studies have generally focused on fish, bird and primate species. Here we extend research on social learning strategies to a taxonomic group that has been neglected until now: otters (subfamily Lutrinae). We collected social association data on captive groups of two gregarious species: smooth-coated otters (Lutrogale perspicillata), known to hunt fish cooperatively in the wild, and Asian short-clawed otters (Aonyx cinereus), which feed individually on prey requiring extractive foraging behaviours. We then presented otter groups with a series of novel foraging tasks, and inferred social transmission of task solutions with network-based diffusion analysis. We show that smooth-coated otters can socially learn how to exploit novel food sources and may adopt a ‘copy when young’ strategy. We found no evidence for social learning in the Asian short-clawed otters. Otters are thus a promising model system for comparative research into social learning strategies, while conservation reintroduction programmes may benefit from facilitating the social transmission of survival skills in these vulnerable species.


Euphytica | 2017

Strategic crossing of biomass and harvest index—source and sink—achieves genetic gains in wheat

Matthew P. Reynolds; Alistair J. D. Pask; William Hoppitt; Kai Sonder; Sivakumar Sukumaran; Gemma Molero; Carolina Saint Pierre; Thomas Payne; Ravi P. Singh; Hans J. Braun; Fernanda G. González; Ignacio I. Terrile; Naresh C. D. Barma; Abdul Hakim; Zhonghu He; Zheru Fan; Dario Novoselovic; Maher Maghraby; Khaled I. M. Gad; ElHusseiny G. Galal; Adel Hagras; Mohamed M. Mohamed; Abdul Fatah A. Morad; Uttam Kumar; Gyanendra Singh; Rudra Naik; Ishwar K. Kalappanavar; Suma S. Biradar; Sakuru V. Sai Prasad; Ravish Chatrath

To accelerate genetic gains in breeding, physiological trait (PT) characterization of candidate parents can help make more strategic crosses, increasing the probability of accumulating favorable alleles compared to crossing relatively uncharacterized lines. In this study, crosses were designed to complement “source” with “sink” traits, where at least one parent was selected for favorable expression of biomass and/or radiation use efficiency—source—and the other for sink-related traits like harvest-index, kernel weight and grains per spike. Female parents were selected from among genetic resources—including landraces and products of wide-crossing (i.e. synthetic wheat)—that had been evaluated in Mexico at high yield potential or under heat stress, while elite lines were used as males. Progeny of crosses were advanced to the F4 generation within Mexico, and F4-derived F5 and F6 generations were yield tested to populate four international nurseries, targeted to high yield environments (2nd and 3rd WYCYT) for yield potential, and heat stressed environments (2nd and 4th SATYN) for climate resilience, respectively. Each nursery was grown as multi-location yield trials. Genetic gains were achieved in both temperate and hot environments, with most new PT-derived lines expressing superior yield and biomass compared to local checks at almost all international sites. Furthermore, the tendency across all four nurseries indicated either the superiority of the best new PT lines compared with the CIMMYT elite checks, or the superiority of all new PT lines as a group compared with all checks, and in some cases, both. Results support—in a realistic breeding context—the hypothesis that yield and radiation use efficiency can be increased by improving source:sink balance, and validate the feasibility of incorporating exotic germplasm into mainstream breeding efforts to accelerate genetic gains for yield potential and climate resilience.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2016

Bayesian Model Selection with Network Based Diffusion Analysis

Andrew Whalen; William Hoppitt

A number of recent studies have used Network Based Diffusion Analysis (NBDA) to detect the role of social transmission in the spread of a novel behavior through a population. In this paper we present a unified framework for performing NBDA in a Bayesian setting, and demonstrate how the Watanabe Akaike Information Criteria (WAIC) can be used for model selection. We present a specific example of applying this method to Time to Acquisition Diffusion Analysis (TADA). To examine the robustness of this technique, we performed a large scale simulation study and found that NBDA using WAIC could recover the correct model of social transmission under a wide range of cases, including under the presence of random effects, individual level variables, and alternative models of social transmission. This work suggests that NBDA is an effective and widely applicable tool for uncovering whether social transmission underpins the spread of a novel behavior, and may still provide accurate results even when key model assumptions are relaxed.


Methods in Ecology and Evolution | 2017

Incorporating intraspecific trait variation into functional diversity: Impacts of selective logging on birds in Borneo

Samuel R. P.‐J. Ross; Christopher Hassall; William Hoppitt; Felicity A. Edwards; David Edwards; Keith C. Hamer

1.As conservation increasingly recognises the importance of species’ functional roles in ecosystem processes, studies are shifting away from measuring species richness towards measures that account for the functional differences between species in a community. These functional diversity (FD) indices have received much recent attention and refinement, but their greatest limitation remains their inability to incorporate information about intraspecific trait variation (ITV). n n2.We use an individual-based model to account for ITV when calculating the functional diversity of two avian communities in Borneo; one in primary (unlogged) forest and one in selectively logged forest. We deal with the scarcity of trait data for individual species by developing a simulation approach, taking data from the literature where necessary. Using a bootstrapping procedure, we produce a range of ecologically feasible FD values taking account of ITV for five commonly-used FD indices, and we quantify the confidence that can be placed in these values using a newly-developed bootstrapping method: btFD. n n3.We found that incorporating ITV significantly altered the FD values of all indices used in our models. The rank order of FD for the two communities, indicating whether diversity was higher in primary or selectively logged forest, was largely unchanged by the inclusion of ITV. However, by accounting for ITV, we were able to reveal previously unrecognized impacts of selective logging on avian functional diversity through a narrower dispersion of individuals in functional trait space in logged forest. n n4.Our results highlight the importance of incorporating ITV into measures of functional diversity, whilst our simulation approach addresses the frequently encountered difficulty of working with sparse trait data and quantifies the confidence that should be placed in such findings. n nThis article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Euphytica | 2018

Correction to: Strategic crossing of biomass and harvest index—source and sink—achieves genetic gains in wheat

Matthew P. Reynolds; Alistair J. D. Pask; William Hoppitt; Kai Sonder; Sivakumar Sukumaran; Gemma Molero; Carolina Saint Pierre; Thomas Payne; Ravi P. Singh; Hans J. Braun; Fernanda G. González; Ignacio I. Terrile; Naresh C. D. Barma; Abdul Hakim; Zhonghu He; Zheru Fan; Dario Novoselovic; Maher Maghraby; Khaled I. M. Gad; ElHusseiny G. Galal; Adel Hagras; Mohamed M. Mohamed; Abdul Fatah A. Morad; Uttam Kumar; Gyanendra Singh; Rudra Naik; Ishwar K. Kalappanavar; Suma S. Biradar; Sakuru V. Sai Prasad; Ravish Chatrath

The original article was corrected. Author Muhammad Kundi should instead read: Muhammad Sohail.


Ibis | 2018

Factors influencing Manx Shearwater grounding on the west coast of Scotland

Martyna Syposz; Filipa Gonçalves; Martin Carty; William Hoppitt; Fabrizio Manco


Archive | 2017

Supplementary material from "Social learning in otters"

Zosia Ladds; William Hoppitt; Neeltje J. Boogert


Archive | 2015

New Caledonian crow social learning apparatus demonstration

Logan Corina; Breen Alexis; Alex H. Taylor; Russell D. Gray; William Hoppitt

Collaboration


Dive into the William Hoppitt's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Zosia Ladds

Anglia Ruskin University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Alistair J. D. Pask

International Maize and Wheat Improvement Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Carolina Saint Pierre

International Maize and Wheat Improvement Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Gemma Molero

International Maize and Wheat Improvement Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Hans J. Braun

International Maize and Wheat Improvement Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kai Sonder

International Maize and Wheat Improvement Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Maher Maghraby

International Maize and Wheat Improvement Center

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge