Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where William M. Hartmann is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by William M. Hartmann.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1982

Localization of sound in rooms

William M. Hartmann

We have studied the ability of human listeners to locate the origin of a sound in a room in a series of source azimuth identification experiments. All experiments were done in a small rectangular concert hall with variable geometry and acoustical properties. Subjects localized a 50‐ms, 500‐Hz sine pulse with an rms error of 3.3° (±0.6°) regardless of room reverberation time. Lowering the ceiling from 11.5 to 3.5 m decreased the error to 2.8° (±0.6°). Subjects localized broadband noise without attack transients with an rms error of 2.3° (±0.6°) if the reverberation time was 1 s. The error increased to 3.2° (±0.7°) if the reverberation time was 5 s. For complex tones without attack transients the localization error continuously increased as the intensity of spectral components decreased. Performance was nearly random for a 500‐Hz sine tone, but was significantly better than random for a 5000‐Hz sine tone. Our azimuth identification experiments revealed significant biases, as much as 2°; such biases are, of ...


Music Perception | 1991

Stream Segregation and Peripheral Channeling

William M. Hartmann; Douglas Johnson

Two interleaved melodies, with theory tones alternating as ABAB..., can be individually followed and identified if auditory stream segregation takes place. Stream segregation can occur if the tone conditions are favorable, for example, if the tones of the different melodies are in different octaves. Using an interleaved melody identification task, we have measured the extent to which 12 different tone conditions lead to stream segregation. The purpose of the experiment is to discover whether stream segregation is mediated entirely by channeling that is established in the auditory periphery or whether more complicated principles of source grouping are at work. Peripheral channels are defined as either tonotopic (frequency based) or lateral (localized left or right). The data show that peripheral channeling is of paramount importance, suggesting that a set of rather simple rules can predict whether two interleaved melodies will be perceived as segregated or not. The data reveal a secondary effect of tone duration. Otherwise, in the absence of peripheral channeling, the experiments find little or no stream segregation, even in those cases where individual tones should clearly evoke images of different sources. Additional experiments show that interleaved melody identification is made more difficult by a transposition that maximizes the number of melodic crossings, even though the transposition may place the interleaved melodies in different keys. An appendix develops an elementary mathematics of melodic crossings and contacts.


Physics Today | 1999

How we localize sound

William M. Hartmann

For as long as we humans have lived on Earth, we have been able to use our ears to localize the sources of sounds. Our ability to localize warns us of danger and helps us sort out individual sounds from the usual cacophony of our acoustical world. Characterizing this ability in humans and other animals makes an intriguing physical, physiological, and psychological study (see figure 1).


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1996

On the externalization of sound images

William M. Hartmann; Andrew T. Wittenberg

Listeners perceive the sounds of the real world to be externalized. The sound images are compact and correctly located in space. The experiments reported in this article attempted to determine the characteristics of signals appearing in the ear canals that are responsible for the perception of externalization. The experiments used headphones to gain experimental control, and they employed a psychophysical method whereby the measurement of externalization was reduced to discrimination. When the headphone signals were synthesized to best resemble real-world signals (the baseline synthesis) listeners could not distinguish between the virtual image created by the headphones and the real source. Externalization was then studied, using both discrimination and listener rating, by systematically modifying the baseline synthesis. It was found that externalization depends on the interaural phases of low-frequency components but not high-frequency components, as defined by a boundary near 1 kHz. By contrast, interaural level differences in all frequency ranges appear to be about equally important. Other experiments showed that externalization requires realistic spectral profiles in both ears; maintaining only the interaural difference spectrum is inadequate. It was also found that externalization does not depend on dispersion around the head; an optimum interaural time difference proved to be an adequate phase relationship.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1985

Localization of sound in rooms, II: The effects of a single reflecting surface

Brad Rakerd; William M. Hartmann

Auditory localization was studied in a room bounded by a single acoustically reflective surface. The position of that surface was varied so as to stimulate a floor, a ceiling, and left and right side walls. The surface was eliminated in one condition so that we could examine localization in free field for purposes of comparison. Using a source identification method we assessed the influences of these various room configurations on the localization of both slow-onset and impulsive sine tones of low frequency (500 Hz). We also measured the steady-state interaural-time-difference (ITD) and interaural-intensity-difference (IID) cues available to subjects in the different room configurations and compared these data with the perceptual judgments. Our results indicate the following: (1) A sound must include transients if the precedence effect is to operate as an aid to its localization in rooms. (2) Even if transients are present the precedence effect does not eliminate all influences of room reflections. (3) Due to the interference of reflections large interaural intensity differences may occur in a room and these have a considerable influence on localization; this is true even at low frequencies for which IID cues do not exist in a free field. (4) Listeners appear to have certain expectations about the reliability and plausibility of various directional cues and perceptually weight the cues accordingly; we suggest that this may explain, in part, the large variation in time-intensity trading ratios reported in the literature and also the differing reports regarding the importance of onsets for localization. (5) In this study we find that onset cues are of some importance to localization even in free field.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1990

Hearing a mistuned harmonic in an otherwise periodic complex tone

William M. Hartmann; Stephen McAdams; Bennett K. Smith

The ability of a listener to detect a mistuned harmonic in an otherwise periodic tone is representative of the capacity to segregate auditory entities on the basis of steady-state signal cues. By use of a task in which listeners matched the pitch of a mistuned harmonic, this ability has been studied, in order to find dependences on mistuned harmonic number, fundamental frequency, signal level, and signal duration. The results considerably augment the data previously obtained from discrimination experiments and from experiments in which listeners counted apparent sources. Although previous work has emphasized the role of spectral resolution in the segregation process, the present work suggests that neural synchrony is an important consideration; our data show that listeners lose the ability to segregate mistuned harmonics at high frequencies where synchronous neural firing vanishes. The functional form of this loss is insensitive to the spacing of the harmonics. The matching experiment also permits the measurement of the pitches of mistuned harmonics. The data exhibit shifts of a form that argues against models of pitch shifts that are based entirely upon partial masking.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1996

Pitch, periodicity, and auditory organization

William M. Hartmann

The perception of pitch forms the basis of musical melody and harmony. It is also among the most precise of all our human senses, and with imagination, this precision can be used experimentally to investigate the functioning of the auditory system. This tutorial presents auditory demonstrations from the zoo of pitch effects: pitch shifts, noise pitch, virtual pitch, dichotic pitch, and the pitches of things that are not there at all. It introduces models of auditory processing, derived from contemporary psychoacoustics and auditory physiology, and tests these models against the experimental effects. It concludes by describing the critical role played by pitch in the important human ability to disentangle overlapping sources of sound.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1988

Noise power fluctuations and the masking of sine signals

William M. Hartmann; Jon Pumplin

This article is concerned with fluctuations in noise power and with the role that such fluctuations play in the masking of sine signals by noise. Several measures of noise fluctuations are discussed: the fourth moment of the waveform, the fourth moment of the envelope, and the crest factor. Relationships among these quantities are found for cases of equal-amplitude random-phase noise and Rayleigh-distributed-amplitude noise. Of particular interest is a special non-Gaussian noise called low-noise noise in which the fluctuations are small by any of our measures. The results of frozen-noise masking experiments are reported, where the noise waveform was fixed for all stimulus presentations. In separate experiments, equal-amplitude random-phase Gaussian noise, with typical fluctuations, and low-noise noise, with almost no fluctuations were used. The data show that for a noise bandwidth less than the critical bandwidth, the masked threshold is about 5 dB lower for low-noise noise than for Gaussian noise. When the noise bandwidth is larger than the critical bandwidth, the masked threshold is the same for both kinds of noise. It is concluded that noise power fluctuations increase masked threshold by about 5 dB and that filtering by the auditory system reintroduces fluctuations into broadband low-noise noise.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1989

Localization of sound in rooms IV: The Franssen effect

William M. Hartmann; Brad Rakerd

The Franssen effect is an illusion that causes human listeners to make large errors in localizing a sound source. This paper describes steps taken to convert the illusion into an experiment in order to study the localization precedence effect as it operates in rooms. The results of the experiment suggest that there are two components to the illusion: The first is the inability of listeners to localize a sine tone in a room in the absence of an onset; the second is the obscuring of modulation cues by the irregular transient response of a room. Experiments show that the Franssen effect fails completely in an anechoic environment, as expected if the effect depends upon the implausibility of steady-state cues in a room. The Franssen effect also fails when the spectrum of the sound is dense.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2013

Human interaural time difference thresholds for sine tones: The high-frequency limit

Andrew Brughera; Larisa Dunai; William M. Hartmann

The smallest detectable interaural time difference (ITD) for sine tones was measured for four human listeners to determine the dependence on tone frequency. At low frequencies, 250-700 Hz, threshold ITDs were approximately inversely proportional to tone frequency. At mid-frequencies, 700-1000 Hz, threshold ITDs were smallest. At high frequencies, above 1000 Hz, thresholds increased faster than exponentially with increasing frequency becoming unmeasurably high just above 1400 Hz. A model for ITD detection began with a biophysically based computational model for a medial superior olive (MSO) neuron that produced robust ITD responses up to 1000 Hz, and demonstrated a dramatic reduction in ITD-dependence from 1000 to 1500 Hz. Rate-ITD functions from the MSO model became inputs to binaural display models-both place based and rate-difference based. A place-based, centroid model with a rigid internal threshold reproduced almost all features of the human data. A signal-detection version of this model reproduced the high-frequency divergence but badly underestimated low-frequency thresholds. A rate-difference model incorporating fast contralateral inhibition reproduced the major features of the human threshold data except for the divergence. A combined, hybrid model could reproduce all the threshold data.

Collaboration


Dive into the William M. Hartmann's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Brad Rakerd

Michigan State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

F. J. Derksen

Michigan State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

J. A. Stick

Michigan State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jon Pumplin

Michigan State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Aimee Shore

Michigan State University

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge