Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Winanda W. Ursinus is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Winanda W. Ursinus.


Journal of Animal Science | 2013

Backtest and novelty behavior of female and castrated male piglets, with diverging social breeding values for growth.

Inonge Reimert; T.B. Rodenburg; Winanda W. Ursinus; N. Duijvesteijn; Irene Camerlink; B. Kemp; J.E. Bolhuis

Pigs housed together in a group influence each others growth. Part of this effect is genetic and can be represented in a social breeding value. It is unknown, however, which traits are associated with social breeding values. The aim of this study was, therefore, to investigate whether personality and response to novelty could be associated with social breeding values for growth in piglets. Female and castrated male piglets from 80 litters, with either an estimated relative positive or negative social breeding value (+SBV or -SBV) for growth, were individually tested in a backtest and novel environment test, and group-wise in a novel object (i.e., a feeder with feed) test and human approach test. All tests were performed during the suckling period. No differences between +SBV and -SBV piglets were found for the frequency and latency of struggling and vocalizing in the backtest (at least, P > 0.30). In the novel object test, piglets with a +SBV for growth touched the feeder faster than piglets with -SBV for growth (P = 0.01) and were more frequently present near the person in the human approach test (P < 0.01). No behavioral differences between +SBV and -SBV piglets were found in the novel environment test (at least, P > 0.40), but piglets that struggled more in the backtest walked more in this test (P = 0.02). Behavior was affected by gender in each test. Female piglets were faster than castrated male piglets to start struggling in the backtest (P = 0.047). In the novel object test, females were faster than males to touch the feeder and sample the feed. In the human approach test, they were also faster than male piglets to touch a person (all, P < 0.001). Females were also more frequently present near the feeder (P < 0.001) and person (P = 0.03). In the novel environment test, female piglets explored the floor more (P = 0.046), produced less low- (P = 0.04) and high-pitched vocalizations (P = 0.02), and defecated (P = 0.08) and urinated less than male piglets (P < 0.01). It was concluded that +SBV and -SBV piglets do not differ in their response to the backtest, and only subtle differences were found in their response to novelty. More research is warranted to identify the traits underlying SBV for growth in pigs. Moreover, castrated male piglets seemed to react more fearfully to each test than female piglets.


PLOS ONE | 2014

Tail Biting in Pigs: Blood Serotonin and Fearfulness as Pieces of the Puzzle?

Winanda W. Ursinus; Cornelis G. van Reenen; Inonge Reimert; J. Elizabeth Bolhuis

Tail biting in pigs is a widespread problem in intensive pig farming. The tendency to develop this damaging behaviour has been suggested to relate to serotonergic functioning and personality characteristics of pigs. We investigated whether tail biting in pigs can be associated with blood serotonin and with their behavioural and physiological responses to novelty. Pigs (n = 480) were born in conventional farrowing pens and after weaning at four weeks of age they were either housed barren (B) or in straw-enriched (E) pens. Individual pigs were exposed to a back test and novel environment test before weaning, and after weaning to a novel object (i.e. bucket) test in an unfamiliar arena. A Principal Component Analysis on behaviours during the tests and salivary cortisol (novel object test only) revealed five factors for both housing systems, labeled ‘Early life exploration’, ‘Near bucket’, ‘Cortisol’, ‘Vocalizations & standing alert’, and ‘Back test activity’. Blood samples were taken at 8, 9 and 22 weeks of age to determine blood platelet serotonin. In different phases of life, pigs were classified as tail biter/non-tail biter based on tail biting behaviour, and as victim/non-victim based on tail wounds. A combination of both classifications resulted in four pig types: biters, victims, biter/victims, and neutrals. Generally, only in phases of life during which pigs were classified as tail biters, they seemed to have lower blood platelet serotonin storage and higher blood platelet uptake velocities. Victims also seemed to have lower blood serotonin storage. Additionally, in B housing, tail biters seemed to consistently have lower scores of the factor ‘Near bucket’, possibly indicating a higher fearfulness in tail biters. Further research is needed to elucidate the nature of the relationship between peripheral 5-HT, fearfulness and tail biting, and to develop successful strategies and interventions to prevent and reduce tail biting.


Journal of Animal Science | 2014

Damaging biting behaviors in intensively kept rearing gilts: The effect of jute sacks and relations with production characteristics

Winanda W. Ursinus; H. J. Wijnen; A. C. Bartels; N. Dijvesteijn; C.G. van Reenen; J.E. Bolhuis

Pigs may display biting behavior directed at pen mates, resulting in body damage such as tail wounds. We assessed the suitability of jute sacks (hung vertically at wall) to reduce biting behaviors and tail wounds in rearing gilts. Additionally, we assessed several characteristics of different types of tail biters. Tail docked rearing gilts originated from 72 litters, which were kept in partly slatted pens with jute sacks (J) or barren control pens (CON; 36 litters per treatment). Tail and ear damage were observed at weaning (4 wk) and during the weaner and rearing phase (17 J and 19 CON pens). Sow (dam) damage was also considered. Biting behaviors (tail, ear, and other) were observed during the weaner and rearing phase. Weight was recorded at birth, weaning, and end of the weaner phase and ADG was calculated from birth till weaning and from weaning till 9 wk. Furthermore, estimated breeding values for litter size, litter birth weight, back fat, and growth between birth and ∼ 105 kg, and ∼ 25 to 105 kg were determined. Jute sacks reduced tail and ear damage at weaning (both P < 0.001) and tended to reduce sow tail damage (P = 0.09). Jute sacks also reduced tail damage post-weaning (P < 0.001 to 0.03), with a 5-fold reduction of (mild) tail wounds at the age of 13 wk. Biting behaviors directed at pen mates were up to 50% lower in J pens (P < 0.001 to 0.04). Higher genotypic litter size, litter birth weight, growth, and lower back fat seemed associated with higher levels of ear or tail-biting behavior (P = 0.002 to 0.09), albeit dependent on treatment and phase. Higher phenotypic litter sizes were associated with higher levels of biting behaviors (P = 0.004-0.08). High-tail-biters and Medium-tail-biters (the latter less pronounced) stemmed from larger litters (P = 0.01 to 0.05), were heavier (P = 0.03 to 0.04), grew faster (P = 0.05 to 0.08), and displayed higher levels of all kinds of biting behavior directed to pen mates and the jute sack (P < 0.001 to 0.10) compared with non-tail-biters, the effect size dependent on treatment and phase of life. To conclude, jute sacks may profoundly reduce damaging behaviors and tail wounds in rearing gilts, probably because they partly meet the behavioral need of pigs for rooting and chewing. Furthermore, (tail) biting is associated with production level of the gilts (phenotypically and genetically), which suggests a role for breeding programs and additional research focusing on metabolic demands of (tail) biting pigs.


Animal | 2017

Heritability of the backtest response in piglets and its genetic correlations with production traits

M.W. Iversen; J.E. Bolhuis; Irene Camerlink; Winanda W. Ursinus; Inonge Reimert; N. Duijvesteijn

The backtest response of a pig gives an indication of its coping style, that is, its preferred strategy to cope with stressful situations, which may in turn be related to production traits. The objective of this study was therefore to estimate the heritability of the backtest response and estimate genetic correlations with production traits (birth weight, growth, fat depth and loin depth). The backtest was performed by placing the piglet on its back for 60 s and the number of struggles (NrS) and vocalizations (NrV), and the latency to struggle and vocalize (LV) was recorded. In total, 992 piglets were subjected to the backtest. Heritability estimates for backtest traits were statistically moderate (although high for behavioral traits), with LV having the highest heritability estimate (0.56±0.10, P<0.001) and NrS having the lowest estimate (0.37±0.09, P<0.001). Backtest traits also had high genetic correlations with each other, with vocalization traits (NrV and LV) having the highest (-0.94±0.03, P<0.001), and NrS with NrV the lowest correlation (0.70±0.09, P<0.001). No significant correlations were found between backtest traits and production traits, but correlations between NrS and birth weight (-0.38±0.25), and NrV and loin depth (-0.28±0.19) approached significance (P=0.07). More research into genotype-by-environment interactions may be needed to assess possible connections between backtest traits and production traits, as this may depend on the circumstances (environment, experiences, etc.). In conclusion, heritability estimates of backtest traits are high and it would therefore be possible to select for them. The high genetic correlations between backtest traits indicate that it may be possible to only consider one or two traits for characterization and selection purposes. There were no significant genetic correlations found between backtest traits and production traits, although some of the correlations approached significance and hence warrant further research.


Behavior Genetics | 2018

Indirect Genetic Effects for Growth in Pigs Affect Behaviour and Weight Around Weaning

Irene Camerlink; Winanda W. Ursinus; Andrea C. Bartels; P. Bijma; J. Elizabeth Bolhuis

Selection for indirect genetic effects (IGE), i.e. the genetic effect of an individual on a trait of another individual, is a promising avenue to increase trait values in plant and animal breeding. Studies in livestock suggest that selection for IGE for growth (IGEg) might increase animals’ capacity to tolerate stress. We assessed the effect of a stressful phase (weaning) on the behaviour and performance of pigs (n = 480) divergently selected for high or low IGEg. High IGEg pigs were significantly slower to explore the feed and gained less weight than low IGEg pigs in the days after weaning. In line with previous findings, high IGEg animals may have prioritized the formation of social ranks.


Proceedings of the 48th Congress of the International Society for Applied Ethology (ISAE) | 2014

Tail biting in pigs: (in)consistency, blood serotonin, and responses to novelty

Winanda W. Ursinus; C.G. van Reenen; Inonge Reimert; B. Kemp; J.E. Bolhuis

Chronic stress is a long-lasting negative emotional state which induces negative consequences on animals’ behavior. This study aimed at assessing whether unpredictable and repeated negative stimuli (URNS) influence feeding behavior in quail, and whether this can be modulated by their emotionality. Two lines of quail divergently selected on their inherent emotionality (low emotionality, STI; high emotionality, LTI) were either daily exposed to URNS or undisturbed from 17 to 40 days of age (n=32 for each line, in each group). During this time, quail were submitted twice to a sequential feeding procedure: they were offered a hypocaloric diet (7% less caloric than the normocaloric diet) on odd days and a hypercaloric diet (7% more caloric) on even days, for 8 days; then, they received a normocaloric diet (metabolizable energy=12.56 MJ) for 3 days. This sequential feeding procedure was used to assess anhedonia and diet preferences thanks to choice tests (hypo vs. hypercaloric diets) performed at the end of each period. Short-term (30 min) and daily intake were also measured each day. Behavioral tests were performed to assess quail’s emotional reactivity. Results showed that URNS enhanced quails’ emotional reactivity, e.g. in the reactivity to human test, disturbed quail came later (P=0.011) and spent less time (P 0.1), but URNS reduced their daily intake during the 2nd period (P<0.05). Motivation for each diet (assessed by their short-term intake) was differently affected by URNS during the 2nd period: STI quail decreased their motivation to eat the hypercaloric diet (P<0.01) whereas LTI increased their motivation to eat the hypocaloric diet (P<0.01). In conclusion, both lines of quail experienced a chronic stress as URNS induced an increase of their emotional reactivity. Interestingly, URNS induced opposite changes in quail’s feeding behavior: LTI disturbed quail seemed to express a short-term compensatory behavior because of their high motivation to eat, whereas STI disturbed quail seemed to be in a devaluation process as shown by their anhedonia and their decrease of daily intake.Farm animals have to adapt to human presence from birth and being handled may lead to fear and stress reactions. It is known that the mother can be used as a postnatal model in the development of young-human relationship. Through her, some information like auditory ones may even be learnt prenatally. We tested this idea in pigs because they communicate a lot by acoustic signals. The hypotheses were that prenatal experience with human voice could modify behavioural reactions to the experienced voice and to an unfamiliar voice expressing different emotions. We worked with 30 pregnant sows from the last month of gestation. Ten sows (treatment A) were submitted to recordings of human voices during handling: vA during positive interactions and vB during negative interactions, twice a day, 5 days a week, for 10 minutes. Ten other sows (treatment B) received the contrary, i.e. vB during positive interactions and vA during negative interactions. Ten last sows (treatment C) received no vocal stimulations during handling sessions. Two days old piglets (36 A, 39 B, 35 C) were submitted to a 5 min choice test between voices vA and vB in a testing pen (2×1 m). Each voice was played back through loudspeakers positioned at each end of the pen. At 15-18 days of age, 20 other piglets from each treatment were tested in the same conditions except that we played back the voice of an unknown person, reading the same text with a joyful or angry intention. In both tests we recorded vocalisations and locomotion. Data were analysed using non parametric statistics (Statview). In both tests, A and B piglets started to move sooner (P 0.05) to be and the time spent (178 s (46s)) close to the loudspeakers did not depend on the treatment (P>0.05). We also found no difference between the time spent close to one loudspeaker or the other, neither for vA versus vB, nor for joyful versus angry intention (P>0.05). The results show that the prenatal experience of human voice reduces postnatal behavioural reactions of stress (vocalisations, latency to move) during the playback human voices. However, it does not seem to induce specific attraction toward human voice, or human emotional intention. Therefore prenatal experience with human voice may be a good way of reducing fear reactions to human voice after birth.


Behavior Genetics | 2015

Indirect Genetic Effects for Growth Rate in Domestic Pigs Alter Aggressive and Manipulative Biting Behaviour

Irene Camerlink; Winanda W. Ursinus; P. Bijma; B. Kemp; J. Elizabeth Bolhuis


Applied Animal Behaviour Science | 2014

Responses to novel situations of female and castrated male pigs with divergent social breeding values and different backtest classifications in barren and straw-enriched housing

Inonge Reimert; T. Bas Rodenburg; Winanda W. Ursinus; B. Kemp; J. Elizabeth Bolhuis


Applied Animal Behaviour Science | 2014

Tail biting behaviour and tail damage in pigs and the relationship with general behaviour: Predicting the inevitable?

Winanda W. Ursinus; Cornelis G. van Reenen; B. Kemp; J. Elizabeth Bolhuis


PLOS ONE | 2014

Selection Based on Indirect Genetic Effects for Growth, Environmental Enrichment and Coping Style Affect the Immune Status of Pigs

Inonge Reimert; T. Bas Rodenburg; Winanda W. Ursinus; B. Kemp; J. Elizabeth Bolhuis

Collaboration


Dive into the Winanda W. Ursinus's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

B. Kemp

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Inonge Reimert

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

J. Elizabeth Bolhuis

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

J.E. Bolhuis

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Cornelis G. van Reenen

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

J.J. Zonderland

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

T. Bas Rodenburg

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge