Alison MacDiarmid
National Institute of Water and Atmospheric Research
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Alison MacDiarmid.
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology | 1999
Alison MacDiarmid; Mark J. Butler
Abstract Sperm limitation, when female fertilisation success is constrained by the supply of sperm, is generally perceived to be an uncommon feature of reproduction in species which directly transfer gametes during copulation. Male size, previous copulations, and the balance of expected reproductive return and future mating opportunity may, however, limit the amount of sperm males transfer to females. We used laboratory experiments where mate size could be manipulated and its consequences on spermatophore size and clutch size determined, to show that in two genera of spiny lobsters (Crustacea: Palinuridae) male reproductive output limits the size of clutches brooded by females. In Panulirus argus from the Florida Keys, we show that while male size affects spermatophore area, males also vary the amount of ejaculate positively with female size. Furthermore, the area of the spermatophore has a greater influence than female size on subsequent clutch weight. In Jasus edwardsii from New Zealand, female size, male size and mate order all affect clutch weight. In both species, clutches fertilised by small males in the laboratory are significantly smaller than clutches fertilised by large males. These results suggest that to ensure they receive sufficient sperm, females should either mate several times prior to oviposition, mate as early as possible in the reproductive season, or choose large, preferably unmated males as partners and thus compete with other females for preferred males. Sperm-limited female fecundity has the potential to limit the egg production of fished populations where large males are typically rare.
New Zealand Journal of Marine and Freshwater Research | 2003
S. Kelly; Alison MacDiarmid
Abstract The maintenance of long‐term associations with particular reef sites is likely to have contributed to the rebuilding of the spiny lobster population (Jasus edwardsii (Hutton) (Decapoda: Palinuridae)) in the Leigh Marine Reserve, in northeast New Zealand. Between 1983 and 1985, 429 lobsters were tagged underwater with western rock lobster tags and antennae tags. Underwater tagging and commercial traps were used to tag a further 737 lobsters with T‐bar tags and antennae tags between 1994 and 1996. Twenty‐one percent of lobsters resighted (n = 323) between 1983 and 1985 maintained their association with a 15 ha reef inside the reserve for 1–8 years. Site association tended to increase with female size, whereas site association in males was relatively constant until 130 mm carapace length, and then markedly increased. Legal‐sized lobster abundance fluctuated seasonally, suggesting that a proportion of the population undertook larger‐scale movements beyond the reef. This was confirmed during a second tagging programme conducted between 1994 and 1996. About 30% of resighted lobsters (n = 212) moved 0.25–6 km from their tagging site and 20% crossed the boundary, either moving into or out of the reserve. These results indicate that although the Leigh Marine Reserve reduces spatial access to fishing grounds, a proportion of the lobster population moves out of the protected area and becomes susceptible to capture in the adjacent fishery.
PLOS ONE | 2010
Dennis P. Gordon; Jennifer Beaumont; Alison MacDiarmid; Donald A. Robertson; Shane T. Ahyong
The marine-biodiversity assessment of New Zealand (Aotearoa as known to Māori) is confined to the 200 nautical-mile boundary of the Exclusive Economic Zone, which, at 4.2 million km2, is one of the largest in the world. It spans 30° of latitude and includes a high diversity of seafloor relief, including a trench 10 km deep. Much of this region remains unexplored biologically, especially the 50% of the EEZ deeper than 2,000 m. Knowledge of the marine biota is based on more than 200 years of marine exploration in the region. The major oceanographic data repository is the National Institute of Water and Atmospheric Research (NIWA), which is involved in several Census of Marine Life field projects and is the location of the Southwestern Pacific Regional OBIS Node; NIWA is also data manager and custodian for fisheries research data owned by the Ministry of Fisheries. Related data sources cover alien species, environmental measures, and historical information. Museum collections in New Zealand hold more than 800,000 registered lots representing several million specimens. During the past decade, 220 taxonomic specialists (85 marine) from 18 countries have been engaged in a project to review New Zealands entire biodiversity. The above-mentioned marine information sources, published literature, and reports were scrutinized to give the results summarized here for the first time (current to 2010), including data on endemism and invasive species. There are 17,135 living species in the EEZ. This diversity includes 4,315 known undescribed species in collections. Species diversity for the most intensively studied phylum-level taxa (Porifera, Cnidaria, Mollusca, Brachiopoda, Bryozoa, Kinorhyncha, Echinodermata, Chordata) is more or less equivalent to that in the ERMS (European Register of Marine Species) region, which is 5.5 times larger in area than the New Zealand EEZ. The implication is that, when all other New Zealand phyla are equally well studied, total marine diversity in the EEZ may be expected to equal that in the ERMS region. This equivalence invites testable hypotheses to explain it. There are 177 naturalized alien species in New Zealand coastal waters, mostly in ports and harbours. Marine-taxonomic expertise in New Zealand covers a broad number of taxa but is, proportionately, at or near its lowest level since the Second World War. Nevertheless, collections are well supported by funding and are continually added to. Threats and protection measures concerning New Zealands marine biodiversity are commented on, along with potential and priorities for future research.
PLOS ONE | 2014
Kathleen Schwerdtner Máñez; Poul Holm; Louise Blight; Marta Coll; Alison MacDiarmid; Henn Ojaveer; Bo Poulsen; Malcolm Tull
Historical research is playing an increasingly important role in marine sciences. Historical data are also used in policy making and marine resource management, and have helped to address the issue of shifting baselines for numerous species and ecosystems. Although many important research questions still remain unanswered, tremendous developments in conceptual and methodological approaches are expected to contribute to a comprehensive understanding of the global history of human interactions with life in the seas. Based on our experiences and knowledge from the “History of Marine Animal Populations” project, this paper identifies the emerging research topics for future historical marine research. It elaborates on concepts and tools which are expected to play a major role in answering these questions, and identifies geographical regions which deserve future attention from marine environmental historians and historical ecologists.
Hormones and Behavior | 2004
Natalie Raethke; Alison MacDiarmid; John C. Montgomery
Chemosensory communication may be crucial during mate choice and mating in the southern temperate spiny lobster Jasus edwardsii to ensure that females mate with large males capable of supplying adequate numbers of sperm during the short mating window. To clarify the role of pheromones during this process, three laboratory experiments were carried out. In an experiment where the output of urine, which contains sex-specific pheromones, from large and small catheterized males was switched, large post-molt females did not make a clear choice of mate. This indicates that while females distinguish among females, males, and juveniles using their chemosensory sense, they distinguish among males using visual and tactile senses in combination with olfaction. Further, two antennule-ablation experiments were carried out to determine if detection of pheromones by the antennules of females or males was critical for mate selection, courting, or mating. In both cases, we observed a (nonsignificant) trend of slightly delayed mating of treatment females. We found that disruption of female olfaction causes less impact on courtship or mating than ablation of male antennules which increased the variance in the length of the period between molting and mating and resulted in a systematic reduction in clutch size. This lesser impact of female ablation may be because females can still respond to their own internal cues about egg ripeness whereas males cannot. In J. edwardsii, unlike the American clawed lobster, Homarus americanus, one fully functional partner of either sex appears sufficient to initiate mating.
New Zealand Journal of Marine and Freshwater Research | 2005
M Oliver; R Stewart; David Mills; Alison MacDiarmid; C Gardner
Abstract The success of enhancement programmes hinges on the survival of released animals. One factor greatly influencing short‐term survival of reseeded lobsters is the timing and intensity of predation relative to the time of release. The activity and abundance of predators varies over daily, seasonal, and annual scales and knowing the best time to release juveniles will minimise mortality. We used chronographic tethering devices and remote video equipment at 10 sites near Wellington, New Zealand and Hobart, Tasmania, Australia, to assess the relative timing and intensity of predation for released lobsters. Our studies showed that predation was greatest within the first 2 h after release (χ 2 = 60.425, d.f. = 9, P < 0.001) suggesting that the disturbance associated with the release itself may draw the unwanted attention of predators. Relative predation rates also peaked on each of the following two mornings, possibly because of the emergence of daytime predators. The video footage obtained at the Tasmanian sites revealed that most predation was by fish (46%), but surprisingly, cannibalism comprised 16% of predation events. The limitations of tethering as a method are discussed in numerous reviews but proved useful as a relative measure for these highly mobile and cryptic animals. Further consideration needs to be given to methods of release that minimise mortality of recently seeded lobsters.
New Zealand Journal of Marine and Freshwater Research | 2009
Darren M. Parsons; Mark A. Morrison; Alison MacDiarmid; Bruce Stirling; Phillip Cleaver; Ian Smith; Maria Butcher
Abstract Anecdotal data sources may constitute an important component of the information available about an exploited species, as record keeping may not have occurred until after exploitation began. Here, we aimed to fill any gaps in the exploitative history of the sparid snapper (Pagrus auratus), using social and historical research methods. Social research consisted of interviews with recreational fishers, focusing on the most and largest snapper they had caught. In addition, the diary‐logs of two recreational fishers were analysed. Historical research consisted of investigation of old books, photos, archives and unpublished sources unconventional to fishery science. Interviews with fishers demonstrated no or weak trends in snapper abundance or size, and were likely impeded by a lack of ability to detect change in a fish stock that may still be considered abundant. The fishers’ perception of change, however, largely reflected recent experiences (last c. 10 years), when biomass is understood to have increased, and mostly did not consider experiences before the 1980s. Alternatively, diary‐logs of fisher catch rates produced a pattern that matched formal stock assessments of snapper biomass, suggesting declines in abundance up until the 1990s and an increase in biomass after that time. Historical research, although more qualitative, had the ability to investigate periods where formal records were not kept and described a fishery vastly different from the current one. Snapper were easily caught, in great abundance and in unusual locations. Localised depletion of snapper was first noticed in the early 20th century, despite spectacular catches of snapper occurring after that time. Snapper behaviour was also likely different, with visual sightings of snapper by onlookers a common occurrence. Although predictions from stock assessment models are consistent with that of the anecdotes listed here (i.e., high biomass in the past), these anecdotes are valuable as they explain lost biomass in a perspective meaningful to all. This perspective may be valuable for managers trying to consider the non‐financial value of a shared fishery but, if unrecognised, represents a shifting baseline.
Reviews in Fisheries Science | 2008
M Oliver; Alison MacDiarmid; R Stewart; C Gardner
Early juvenile lobsters reared in captivity may loose anti-predator behaviors displayed by their wild counterparts. To test this hypothesis in juvenile spiny lobsters (Jasus edwardsii), we conducted a series of experiments in which recently settled pueruli were reared in captivity under differing levels of predation risk. After one year in captivity, these lobsters were exposed to a predator in a tank or released into the wild to assess their ability to recognize and respond to predation. We found that lobsters raised without predators significantly reduced their movement activity outside shelters when subsequently introduced to a predator compared with juveniles raised with predators (F1,45 = 4.33, p < 0.05). This apparent over-compensation may be necessary for lobsters to learn the appropriate anti-predator response. Lobsters raised without predators and released into the wild displayed the same types of behaviors as resident wild lobsters. Released lobsters spent more time defending and fighting amongst themselves than the wild lobsters (G = 13.006 > χ2 0.05,2 = 9.488), but they displayed the appropriate anti-predator responses when approached or attacked by a predator. Our results show that juvenile spiny lobsters habituate to the absence of a predator when reared in captivity for long periods, but display an innate ability to recognize and effectively respond to predators when encountered in a hatchery environment or in the wild.
Environmental Conservation | 2012
Debbie J. Freeman; Alison MacDiarmid; Richard B. Taylor; Robert J. Davidson; Roger Grace; Tim R. Haggitt; Shane Kelly
DEBBIE J . FREEMAN 1 , 2 ∗, ALISON B . MACDIARMID 3 , RICHARD B . TAYLOR 1 , ROBERT J. D AVIDSON 4 , ROGER V. GRACE 5 , T I M R. H A G G I T T 6 , SHANE KELLY 7 A ND N ICK T . SHE AR S 8 1University of Auckland, Leigh Marine Laboratory, PO Box 349, Warkworth 0941, New Zealand, 2Department of Conservation, PO Box 10-420, Wellington 6143, New Zealand, 3National Institute of Water and Atmospheric Research, PO Box 14-901, Kilbirnie, Wellington 6241, New Zealand, 4Davidson Environmental Ltd, PO Box 958, Nelson 7040, New Zealand, 5Mountains to Sea Conservation Trust, c/o 539 Rockell Road, RD1, Hikurangi, Whangarei 0181, New Zealand, 6Coastal and Aquatic Systems Ltd, PO Box 54, Leigh 0947, New Zealand, 7Coast and Catchment Ltd, 3 The Brae, Maraetai Beach, Manukau City 2018, New Zealand, and 8University of Auckland, Department of Statistics, Private Bag 92019, Auckland 1142, New Zealand Date submitted: 19 July 2011; Date accepted: 10 April 2012
New Zealand Journal of Marine and Freshwater Research | 2006
M Oliver; Alison MacDiarmid; R Stewart; C Gardner
Abstract Lobster stock enhancement efforts have generally met with limited success owing, in part, to low survival of reseeded juveniles caused by poor development of anti‐predator behaviour. We found that recently‐settled juvenile lobsters reared in captivity for 1 year exhibit significantly higher levels of daytime activity than their like‐sized wild counterparts and that this daytime activity could be reduced by 50% by either rearing the lobsters with a predator or by feeding them at night. In combination, predator presence and night‐time feeding further reduced daytime emergence to low levels. However, a field experiment in which we observed the behaviour and emergence times of captive‐reared lobsters immediately after release on reefs where predators were abundant, showed that regardless of rearing conditions and associated changes in behaviour, lobsters assess the risk of predation and quickly assume nocturnal emergence and activity patterns. This plasticity in behaviour may enable juveniles to maximise foraging range and duration in response to predation pressure.