Alma Veenstra
Max Planck Society
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Alma Veenstra.
Language Acquisition | 2016
Spyridoula Varlokosta; Adriana Belletti; João Costa; Naama Friedmann; Anna Gavarró; Kleanthes K. Grohmann; Maria Teresa Guasti; Laurice Tuller; Maria Lobo; Darinka Anđelković; Núria Argemí; Larisa Avram; Sanne Berends; Valentina Brunetto; Hélène Delage; Maria-José Ezeizabarrena; Iris Fattal; Ewa Haman; Angeliek van Hout; Kristine M. Jensen de López; Napoleon Katsos; Lana Kologranic; Nadezda Krstić; Jelena Kuvač Kraljević; Aneta Miękisz; Michaela Nerantzini; Clara Queraltó; Zeljana Radic; Sílvia Ruiz; Uli Sauerland
ABSTRACT This study develops a single elicitation method to test the acquisition of third-person pronominal objects in 5-year-olds for 16 languages. This methodology allows us to compare the acquisition of pronominals in languages that lack object clitics (“pronoun languages”) with languages that employ clitics in the relevant context (“clitic languages”), thus establishing a robust cross-linguistic baseline in the domain of clitic and pronoun production for 5-year-olds. High rates of pronominal production are found in our results, indicating that children have the relevant pragmatic knowledge required to select a pronominal in the discourse setting involved in the experiment as well as the relevant morphosyntactic knowledge involved in the production of pronominals. It is legitimate to conclude from our data that a child who at age 5 is not able to produce any or few pronominals is a child at risk for language impairment. In this way, pronominal production can be taken as a developmental marker, provided that one takes into account certain cross-linguistic differences discussed in the article.
Language, cognition and neuroscience | 2014
Alma Veenstra; Daniel J. Acheson; Kathryn Bock; Antje S. Meyer
The generation of subject–verb agreement is a central component of grammatical encoding. It is sensitive to conceptual and grammatical influences, but the interplay between these factors is still not fully understood. We investigate how semantic integration of the subject noun phrase (‘the secretary of/with the governor’) and the Local Noun Number (‘the secretary with the governor/governors’) affect the ease of selecting the verb form. Two hypotheses are assessed: according to the notional hypothesis, integration encourages the assignment of the singular notional number to the noun phrase and facilitates the choice of the singular verb form. According to the lexical interference hypothesis, integration strengthens the competition between nouns within the subject phrase, making it harder to select the verb form when the nouns mismatch in number. In two experiments, adult speakers of Dutch completed spoken preambles (Experiment 1) or selected appropriate verb forms (Experiment 2). Results showed facilitatory effects of semantic integration (fewer errors and faster responses with increasing integration). These effects did not interact with the effects of the Local Noun Number (slower response times and higher error rates for mismatching than for matching noun numbers). The findings thus support the notional hypothesis and a model of agreement where conceptual and lexical factors independently contribute to the determination of the number of the subject noun phrase and, ultimately, the verb.
Frontiers in Psychology | 2014
Alma Veenstra; Daniel J. Acheson; Antje S. Meyer
Compared to the large body of work on lexical access, little research has been done on grammatical encoding in language production. An exception is the generation of subject-verb agreement. Here, two key findings have been reported: (1) speakers make more agreement errors when the head and local noun of a phrase mismatch in number than when they match [e.g., the key to the cabinet(s)]; and (2) this attraction effect is asymmetric, with stronger attraction for singular than for plural head nouns. Although these findings are robust, the cognitive processes leading to agreement errors and their significance for the generation of correct agreement are not fully understood. We propose that future studies of agreement, and grammatical encoding in general, may benefit from using paradigms that tightly control the variability of the lexical content of the material. We report two experiments illustrating this approach. In both of them, the experimental items featured combinations of four nouns, four color adjectives, and two prepositions. In Experiment 1, native speakers of Dutch described pictures in sentences such as the circle next to the stars is blue. In Experiment 2, they carried out a forced-choice task, where they read subject noun phrases (e.g., the circle next to the stars) and selected the correct verb-phrase (is blue or are blue) with a button press. Both experiments showed an attraction effect, with more errors after subject phrases with mismatching, compared to matching head and local nouns. This effect was stronger for singular than plural heads, replicating the attraction asymmetry. In contrast, the response times recorded in Experiment 2 showed similar attraction effects for singular and plural head nouns. These results demonstrate that critical agreement phenomena can be elicited reliably in lexically reduced contexts. We discuss the theoretical implications of the findings and the potential and limitations of studies using lexically simple materials.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2018
Alma Veenstra; Kyriakos Antoniou; Napoleon Katsos; Mikhail Kissine
We propose that attraction errors in agreement production (e.g., the key to the cabinets are missing) are related to two components of executive control: working memory and inhibitory control. We tested 138 children aged 10 to 12, an age when children are expected to produce high rates of errors. To increase the potential of individual variation in executive control skills, participants came from monolingual, bilingual, and bidialectal language backgrounds. Attraction errors were elicited with a picture description task in Dutch and executive control was measured with a digit span task, Corsi blocks task, switching task, and attentional networks task. Overall, higher rates of attraction errors were negatively associated with higher verbal working memory and, independently, with higher inhibitory control. To our knowledge, this is the first demonstration of the role of both working memory and inhibitory control in attraction errors in production. Implications for memory- and grammar-based models are discussed.
the 4th Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America (GALANA 2010) | 2011
Angeliek van Hout; Alma Veenstra; Sanne Berends
Groninger Arbeiten zur germanistischen Linguistik | 2010
Sanne Berends; Alma Veenstra; Angeliek van Hout
Language and Cognitive Processes | 2013
Alma Veenstra; Daniel J. Acheson; J. K. Bock; Antje S. Meyer
Archive | 2018
Alma Veenstra; Napoleon Katsos
BUCLD 41: Proceedings of the 41st annual Boston University Conference on Language Development | 2017
Alma Veenstra; Kyriakos Antoniu; Napoleon Katsos; Mikhail Kissine
the Sixth Annual Meeting of the Society for the Neurobiology of Language (SNL 2014) | 2014
Daniel J. Acheson; Alma Veenstra; Antje S. Meyer; Peter Hagoort