Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Andreas Lind is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Andreas Lind.


Consciousness and Cognition | 2006

How something can be said about telling more than we can know: On choice blindness and introspection

Petter Johansson; Lars Hall; Sverker Sikström; Betty Tärning; Andreas Lind

The legacy of Nisbett and Wilsons classic article, Telling More Than We Can Know: Verbal Reports on Mental Processes (1977), is mixed. It is perhaps the most cited article in the recent history of consciousness studies, yet no empirical research program currently exists that continues the work presented in the article. To remedy this, we have introduced an experimental paradigm we call choice blindness [Johansson, P., Hall, L., Sikström, S., & Olsson, A. (2005). Failure to detect mismatches between intention and outcome in a simple decision task. Science, 310(5745), 116-119.]. In the choice blindness paradigm participants fail to notice mismatches between their intended choice and the outcome they are presented with, while nevertheless offering introspectively derived reasons for why they chose the way they did. In this article, we use word-frequency and latent semantic analysis (LSA) to investigate a corpus of introspective reports collected within the choice blindness paradigm. We contrast the introspective reasons given in non-manipulated vs. manipulated trials, but find very few differences between these two groups of reports.


PLOS ONE | 2013

How the polls can be both spot on and dead wrong: using choice blindness to shift political attitudes and voter intentions.

Lars Hall; Thomas Strandberg; Philip Pärnamets; Andreas Lind; Betty Tärning; Petter Johansson

Political candidates often believe they must focus their campaign efforts on a small number of swing voters open for ideological change. Based on the wisdom of opinion polls, this might seem like a good idea. But do most voters really hold their political attitudes so firmly that they are unreceptive to persuasion? We tested this premise during the most recent general election in Sweden, in which a left- and a right-wing coalition were locked in a close race. We asked our participants to state their voter intention, and presented them with a political survey of wedge issues between the two coalitions. Using a sleight-of-hand we then altered their replies to place them in the opposite political camp, and invited them to reason about their attitudes on the manipulated issues. Finally, we summarized their survey score, and asked for their voter intention again. The results showed that no more than 22% of the manipulated replies were detected, and that a full 92% of the participants accepted and endorsed our altered political survey score. Furthermore, the final voter intention question indicated that as many as 48% (±9.2%) were willing to consider a left-right coalition shift. This can be contrasted with the established polls tracking the Swedish election, which registered maximally 10% voters open for a swing. Our results indicate that political attitudes and partisan divisions can be far more flexible than what is assumed by the polls, and that people can reason about the factual issues of the campaign with considerable openness to change.


Psychological Science | 2014

Speakers’ Acceptance of Real-Time Speech Exchange Indicates That We Use Auditory Feedback to Specify the Meaning of What We Say

Andreas Lind; Lars Hall; Björn Breidegard; Christian Balkenius; Petter Johansson

Speech is usually assumed to start with a clearly defined preverbal message, which provides a benchmark for self-monitoring and a robust sense of agency for one’s utterances. However, an alternative hypothesis states that speakers often have no detailed preview of what they are about to say, and that they instead use auditory feedback to infer the meaning of their words. In the experiment reported here, participants performed a Stroop color-naming task while we covertly manipulated their auditory feedback in real time so that they said one thing but heard themselves saying something else. Under ideal timing conditions, two thirds of these semantic exchanges went undetected by the participants, and in 85% of all nondetected exchanges, the inserted words were experienced as self-produced. These findings indicate that the sense of agency for speech has a strong inferential component, and that auditory feedback of one’s own voice acts as a pathway for semantic monitoring, potentially overriding other feedback loops.


Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | 2014

Auditory feedback of one's own voice is used for high-level semantic monitoring: the "self-comprehension" hypothesis.

Andreas Lind; Lars Hall; Björn Breidegard; Christian Balkenius; Petter Johansson

What would it be like if we said one thing, and heard ourselves saying something else? Would we notice something was wrong? Or would we believe we said the thing we heard? Is feedback of our own speech only used to detect errors, or does it also help to specify the meaning of what we say? Comparator models of self-monitoring favor the first alternative, and hold that our sense of agency is given by the comparison between intentions and outcomes, while inferential models argue that agency is a more fluent construct, dependent on contextual inferences about the most likely cause of an action. In this paper, we present a theory about the use of feedback during speech. Specifically, we discuss inferential models of speech production that question the standard comparator assumption that the meaning of our utterances is fully specified before articulation. We then argue that auditory feedback provides speakers with a channel for high-level, semantic “self-comprehension”. In support of this we discuss results using a method we recently developed called Real-time Speech Exchange (RSE). In our first study using RSE (Lind et al., in press) participants were fitted with headsets and performed a computerized Stroop task. We surreptitiously recorded words they said, and later in the test we played them back at the exact same time that the participants uttered something else, while blocking the actual feedback of their voice. Thus, participants said one thing, but heard themselves saying something else. The results showed that when timing conditions were ideal, more than two thirds of the manipulations went undetected. Crucially, in a large proportion of the non-detected manipulated trials, the inserted words were experienced as self-produced by the participants. This indicates that our sense of agency for speech has a strong inferential component, and that auditory feedback of our own voice acts as a pathway for semantic monitoring. We believe RSE holds great promise as a tool for investigating the role of auditory feedback during speech, and we suggest a number of future studies to serve this purpose.


Behavior Research Methods | 2018

DAVID: An open-source platform for real-time transformation of infra-segmental emotional cues in running speech

Laura Rachman; Marco Liuni; Pablo Arias; Andreas Lind; Petter Johansson; Lars Hall; Daniel C. Richardson; Katsumi Watanabe; Stéphanie Dubal; Jean-Julien Aucouturier

We present an open-source software platform that transforms emotional cues expressed by speech signals using audio effects like pitch shifting, inflection, vibrato, and filtering. The emotional transformations can be applied to any audio file, but can also run in real time, using live input from a microphone, with less than 20-ms latency. We anticipate that this tool will be useful for the study of emotions in psychology and neuroscience, because it enables a high level of control over the acoustical and emotional content of experimental stimuli in a variety of laboratory situations, including real-time social situations. We present here results of a series of validation experiments aiming to position the tool against several methodological requirements: that transformed emotions be recognized at above-chance levels, valid in several languages (French, English, Swedish, and Japanese) and with a naturalness comparable to natural speech.


Consciousness and Cognition | 2006

Reply to commentary by Moore and Haggard

Lars Hall; Petter Johansson; Sverker Sikström; Betty Tärning; Andreas Lind

We are very happy to see that Moore and Haggard (2006) welcome the introduction of CBP as a usefulexperimental method for investigating introspection and intentionality, but while they urge caution in theextent of the application of our method, we can do nothing but energetically encourage its use. When Mooreand Haggard write ‘‘in line with Nisbett and Wilson’s hypothesis, the CBP suggests that our introspections areconfabulatory’’, they are not entirely correct. The results of the studies we have done so far using the CPBsuggest that introspections about (some forms of) decisions may (sometimes) be confabulatory. But the par-adigm itself is neutral about this point. In fact, from an analytic perspective we would have preferred to findclear patterns of differences between the NM- and M-reports, because that would have allowed us to startbuilding up a contrast case for different modes of introspective reporting, and to eventually perhaps arriveat a powerful generalization about truthful and confabulatory content. Now, as Moore and Haggard note,we have a more sweeping and difficult hypothesis to test in further experiments, namely that the NM-reportsmay contain lots of confabulatory elements too.What would it mean if this hypothesis were true? We suspect that part of the caution urged by Moore andHaggard about the CBP lies in a general worry that overstating the conclusions of the present findings coulddo wrongful damage to the image we have of ourselves as insightful and rational creatures. However, we feel itis unfortunate that efforts like those of Nisbett and Wilson (1977) and Wegner (2002) often get bundled withthe idea of a demotion of the powers of the human mind. They (and we) are not here to con people or tomanipulate them, but to map out the relationship between the concepts of everyday psychology and scientifictheories of introspection and intentionality. As Dennett (1987) writes:Wewouldbeunwisetomodelourscientificpsychologytoocloselyontheseputativeillata(concreteentities)offolktheory.Wepostulatealltheseapparentactivitiesandmentalprocessesinordertomakesenseofthebehaviorweobserve—inorder,infact,tomakeasmuchsensepossibleofthebehavior,especiallywhenthebehavior we observe is our own...each of us is in most regards a sort of inveterate auto-psychologist,effortlesslyinventingintentionalinterpretationsofourownactionsinaninseparablemixofconfabulation,retrospective self-justification, and (on occasion, no doubt) good theorizing. (p. 91, emphasis in original).


Psychological Science | 2015

Auditory Feedback Is Used for Self-Comprehension: When We Hear Ourselves Saying Something Other Than What We Said, We Believe We Said What We Hear

Andreas Lind; Lars Hall; Björn Breidegard; Christian Balkenius; Petter Johansson

Auditory Feedback Is Used for Self-Comprehension : When We Hear Ourselves Saying Something Other Than What We Said, We Believe We Said What We Hear


bioRxiv | 2017

DAVID: An open-source platform for real-time emotional speech transformation With 25 applications in the behavioral sciences

Laura Rachman; Marco Liuni; Pablo Arias; Andreas Lind; Petter Johansson; Lars Hall; Daniel C. Richardson; Katsumi Watanabe; Stéphanie Dubal; Jean-Julien Aucouturier

We present an open-source software platform that transforms the emotions expressed by speech signals using audio effects like pitch shifting, inflection, vibrato, and filtering. The emotional transformations can be applied to any audio file, but can also run in real-time (with less than 20-millisecond latency), using live input from a microphone. We anticipate that this tool will be useful for the study of emotions in psychology and neuroscience, because it enables a high level of control over the acoustical and emotional content of experimental stimuli in a variety of laboratory situations, including real-time social situations. We present here results of a series of validation experiments showing that transformed emotions are recognized at above-chance levels in the French, English, Swedish and Japanese languages, with a naturalness comparable to natural speech. Then, we provide a list of twenty-five experimental ideas applying this new tool to important topics in the behavioral sciences.


Theoria | 2008

Particularism in question: an interview with Jonathan Dancy

Andreas Lind; Johan Brännmark


Lund University Cognitive Studies; 158 (2014) | 2014

Semantic Self-monitoring in Speech. Using Real-time Speech Exchange to Investigate the Use of Auditory Feedback for Self-comprehension

Andreas Lind

Collaboration


Dive into the Andreas Lind's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge