Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Anette Wichman is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Anette Wichman.


PLOS ONE | 2007

Transmission of stress-induced learning impairment and associated brain gene expression from parents to offspring in chickens.

Christina Lindqvist; Andrew M. Janczak; Daniel Nätt; Izabella Baranowska; Niclas Lindqvist; Anette Wichman; Joakim Lundeberg; Johan Lindberg; Peter A. Torjesen; Per Jensen

Background Stress influences many aspects of animal behaviour and is a major factor driving populations to adapt to changing living conditions, such as during domestication. Stress can affect offspring through non-genetic mechanisms, but recent research indicates that inherited epigenetic modifications of the genome could possibly also be involved. Methodology/Principal Findings Red junglefowl (RJF, ancestors of modern chickens) and domesticated White Leghorn (WL) chickens were raised in a stressful environment (unpredictable light-dark rhythm) and control animals in similar pens, but on a 12/12 h light-dark rhythm. WL in both treatments had poorer spatial learning ability than RJF, and in both populations, stress caused a reduced ability to solve a spatial learning task. Offspring of stressed WL, but not RJF, raised without parental contact, had a reduced spatial learning ability compared to offspring of non-stressed animals in a similar test as that used for their parents. Offspring of stressed WL were also more competitive and grew faster than offspring of non-stressed parents. Using a whole-genome cDNA microarray, we found that in WL, the same changes in hypothalamic gene expression profile caused by stress in the parents were also found in the offspring. In offspring of stressed WL, at least 31 genes were up- or down-regulated in the hypothalamus and pituitary compared to offspring of non-stressed parents. Conclusions/Significance Our results suggest that, in WL the gene expression response to stress, as well as some behavioural stress responses, were transmitted across generations. The ability to transmit epigenetic information and behaviour modifications between generations may therefore have been favoured by domestication. The mechanisms involved remain to be investigated; epigenetic modifications could either have been inherited or acquired de novo in the specific egg environment. In both cases, this would offer a novel explanation to rapid evolutionary adaptation of a population.


Laterality | 2009

Light exposure during incubation and social and vigilance behaviour of domestic chicks

Anette Wichman; Rafael Freire; Lesley J. Rogers

Light exposure of chick eggs during a sensitive period at the end of the incubation period leads to the development of lateralised visual behaviour, and here we show that social behaviour is also influenced by this exposure. Groups of eight chicks of three types—(1) all incubated in the dark (Da), (2) all exposed to light (Li), (3) half Da and half Li (Mixed)—were tested on a range of tasks involving social competition and vigilance for a simulated predator. We confirmed a previous finding that lowest-ranking chicks in Li groups gained less access to a food bowl when they had to compete with group members than did the lowest-ranking chicks in the Da groups, and we extended this result to show that the Mixed groups performed like the Li groups. We also showed that before presentation of a novel stimulus resembling a predator, fewer of the chicks in the Da groups than in the Li and Mixed groups looked up, but during presentation of the predator more Da than Li chicks looked up. Hence, light exposure before hatching affects post-hatching social and vigilance behaviour.


Behavioural Processes | 2009

Visual lateralization and development of spatial and social spacing behaviour of chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus).

Anette Wichman; Lesley J. Rogers; Rafael Freire

We investigated whether the development of spatial behaviour of the domestic chicken is influenced by light exposure of the embryo, as is known to be the case for some other lateralized visual functions. Ninety-six chicks were incubated in the dark or exposed to light during the final days of incubation. Half of the chicks in each group had the experience of moving behind opaque screens from 10 to 12 days of age. The other half were given transparent screens as a control. Chicks were tested in a detour test and a rotated floor test and their dispersal in groups was observed in larger pens. In the rotated floor test, chicks that had had experience with opaque screens used distal cues significantly more often than chicks that had experience with transparent screens (P=0.042), regardless of whether they had been exposed to light before hatching or incubated in the dark. There were no significant differences between treatments in the detour test or in the dispersal behaviour. Hence, visual lateralization has no influence on the development of the spatial behaviour that we tested, whereas the occlusion experience is quite specific and results in shifted attention to distal spatial cues.


Scientific Reports | 2018

Environmental complexity buffers against stress-induced negative judgement bias in female chickens

Josefina Zidar; Irene Campderrich; Emelie Jansson; Anette Wichman; Svante Winberg; Linda J. Keeling; Hanne Løvlie

Cognitive processes are often biased by emotions. In humans, affective disorders are accompanied by pessimistic judgement, while optimistic judgement is linked to emotional stability. Similar to humans, animals tend to interpret ambiguous stimuli negatively after experiencing stressful events, although the long-lasting impact on judgement bias has rarely been investigated. We measure judgement bias in female chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus) after exposure to cold stress, and before and after exposure to additional unpredictable stressors. Additionally, we explore if brain monoamines can explain differences in judgement bias. Chicks exposed to cold stress did not differ in judgement bias compared to controls, but showed sensitivity to additional stressors by having higher motivation for social reinstatement. Environmental complexity reduced stress-induced negative judgement bias, by maintaining an optimistic bias in individuals housed in complex conditions even after stress exposure. Moreover, judgement bias was related to dopamine turnover rate in mesencephalon, with higher activity in individuals that had a more optimistic response. These results demonstrate that environmental complexity can buffer against negative effects of additive stress and that dopamine relates to judgement bias in chicks. These results reveal that both internal and external factors can mediate emotionally biased judgement in animals, thus showing similarities to findings in humans.


Frontiers in Veterinary Science | 2018

Ability of Laying Hens to Distinguish Between Companions According to Their Success in Gaining Access to Food

Anette Wichman

Laying hens (Gallus gallus) are social birds with cognitive abilities related to having a functional interaction with their peers. Gaining knowledge about for example new food sources from other individuals can be a valuable complement to individual learning and probably even more so if one copies the behavior of successful individuals. In this study the aim was to investigate if a bird would identify another bird as being successful at gaining access to food. A social cognition feeding test was developed where birds could move freely together between several scattered food sources. Two different methods were used for training. In method 1, the observer hens were exposed to a skilled demonstrator hen that gained access to the food sources and an unskilled demonstrator hen (that gained no access to food) at the same time when trained together in a trio. In method 2, the observer was trained in two different pair constellations, with a skilled and unskilled demonstrator, respectively. In the test situation for both methods birds were paired, one observer was tested once with the skilled demonstrator and once with the unskilled demonstrator. Observations of how much the observer birds followed the two different demonstrators to the food sources, although no food was available during testing, were carried out. Observers trained in trios (method 1) did not show any difference in following behavior between the skilled and unskilled demonstrator, but observers that had been trained in pairs (method 2) showed more following behavior toward the skilled demonstrators than the unskilled demonstrator (P = 0.005). Thus the results indicate that laying hens are able to use another bird as a cue of whether they will get access to food.


Applied Animal Behaviour Science | 2007

Effects of broody hens on perch use, ground pecking, feather pecking and cannibalism in domestic fowl (Gallus gallus domesticus)

Anja B. Riber; Anette Wichman; Bjarne O. Braastad; Björn Forkman


Applied Animal Behaviour Science | 2006

Development of perching behaviour in chicks reared in enriched environment

Matti Heikkilä; Anette Wichman; Stefan Gunnarsson; Anna Valros


Livestock Science | 2009

Influence of sow dietary fatty acid composition on the behaviour of the piglets

Stefan Gunnarsson; Jana Pickova; Anders Högberg; Maria Neil; Anette Wichman; Isabelle Wigren; Kerstin Uvnäs-Moberg; L. Rydhmer


Journal of The American Association for Laboratory Animal Science | 2018

Methods for Investigating the Motivation of Mice to Explore and Access Food Rewards

Elin Spangenberg; Anette Wichman


Anaporc: revista de la Asociación de Porcinocultura Científica | 2009

Influencia de la composición de los ácidos grasos en la dieta de la cerda sobre el comportamiento de los lechones

Stefan Gunnarsson; Jana Pickova; Anders Högberg; Maria Neil; Anette Wichman; Isabelle Wigren; Kerstin Uvnäs-Moberg; L. Rydhmer; Pedro Sánchez Mampel

Collaboration


Dive into the Anette Wichman's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Stefan Gunnarsson

Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jana Pickova

Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kerstin Uvnäs-Moberg

Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

L. Rydhmer

Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Maria Neil

Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Rafael Freire

Charles Sturt University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Anna Valros

University of Helsinki

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge