Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Belén López-Pérez is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Belén López-Pérez.


Basic and Applied Social Psychology | 2014

Empathy, awareness of others, and action: How feeling empathy for one-among-others motivates helping the others

Luis Oceja; Marc W. Heerdink; Eric L. Stocks; Tamara Ambrona; Belén López-Pérez; Sergio Salgado

Feeling empathy for a member of the group may result in either favoring this individual at the expense of the group or helping the entire group. We explain these intriguing findings by proposing that the combined influence of feeling empathy for one individual and awareness of others enhances willingness to help both the individual and the others (taken as individuals). The results of three experiments showed that inducing empathy for one individual promotes favoring him or her at the expense of the group, whereas inducing empathy for one-among-others leads to helping these others individually, instead of as a group. Furthermore, the awareness of others mediated the proposed one-among-others effect.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2015

Parent–child discrepancies in the assessment of children’s and adolescents’ happiness

Belén López-Pérez; Ellie L. Wilson

In this study, we assessed parent-child agreement in the perception of childrens general happiness or well-being in typically developing children (10- and 11-year-olds, n = 172) and adolescents (15- and 16-year-olds, n = 185). Despite parent and child reporters providing internally consistent responses in the General Happiness single-item scale and the Oxford Happiness Questionnaire-Short Form, their perceptions about childrens and adolescents general happiness did not correlate. Parents of 10- and 11-year-olds significantly overestimated childrens happiness, supporting previous literature on the parents positivity bias effect. However, parents of 15- and 16-year-olds showed the reverse pattern by underestimating adolescents happiness. Furthermore, parents self-reported happiness or well-being (reported 6 months later) significantly correlated with their estimations of childrens and adolescents happiness. Therefore, these results suggest a potential parents egocentric bias when estimating their childrens happiness. These findings are discussed in terms of their theoretical and applied implications for research into child-parent relationships.


Psychological Science | 2017

Cruel to Be Kind: Factors Underlying Altruistic Efforts to Worsen Another Person’s Mood

Belén López-Pérez; Laura Howells; Michaela Gummerum

When aiming to improve another person’s long-term well-being, people may choose to induce a negative emotion in that person in the short term. We labeled this form of agent-target interpersonal emotion regulation altruistic affect worsening and hypothesized that it may happen when three conditions are met: (a) The agent experiences empathic concern for the target of the affect-worsening process, (b) the negative emotion to be induced helps the target achieve a goal (e.g., anger for confrontation or fear for avoidance), and (c) there is no benefit for the agent. This hypothesis was tested by manipulating perspective-taking instructions and the goal to be achieved while participants (N = 140) played a computer-based video game. Participants following other-oriented perspective-taking instructions, compared with those following objective perspective-taking instructions, decided to induce more anger in a supposed fellow participant who was working to achieve a confrontation goal and to induce more fear in a supposed fellow participant who was working to achieve an avoidance goal.


Journal of Health Psychology | 2017

Predicting prostate cancer treatment choices: The role of numeracy, time discounting, and risk attitudes.

Belén López-Pérez; Andrew J. Barnes; Dominick L. Frosch; Yaniv Hanoch

Prostate cancer is the most common cancer among males in the United States, and there is lack of consensus as to whether active surveillance or radical prostatectomy is the best course of treatment. In this study, we examined the role of numeracy, time discounting, and risk taking on decision-making about prostate cancer treatment—in 279 men over age 50 without a prior prostate cancer diagnosis. Results showed that active surveillance was the most chosen option and its preference was predicted by numeracy and time discounting. However, radical prostatectomy was significantly predicted by participants’ propensity to take risks.


Social Science Journal | 2014

Testing the qualitative differences between empathy and personal distress: Measuring core affect and self-orientation

Belén López-Pérez; Pilar Carrera; Tamara Ambrona; Luis Oceja

Abstract In two studies the emotional valence, the level of arousal and self-orientation of empathic concern and personal distress are tested. The empathic concern prevalence versus personal distress prevalence is induced through pictures in Study 1 (N = 62) and perspective-taking instructions in Study 2 (N = 60). Results of Study 1 show that participants in the personal distress-prevalence condition report significantly more negative emotional valence (d = .65), higher arousal (d = .48), and higher self-orientation (d = .57) than participants in the empathic concern-prevalence condition. Results of Study 2 show that participants in the self-oriented condition report the highest arousal, followed by participants in the other-oriented condition, and the objective condition (ηp2 = .58). Concerning the increase of self-orientation, results show that it is mediated by the personal distress induced by self-perspective instructions. Similar results in emotional valence were found between self- and other-oriented conditions.


Journal of Interpersonal Violence | 2017

Cognitive and Affective Empathy, Personal Belief in a Just World, and Bullying Among Offenders

Belén López-Pérez; Yaniv Hanoch; Kayleigh Holt; Michaela Gummerum

Bullying extracts a heavy toll on offenders and prison staff alike. Studying what factors may affect bullying is extremely important as this may help to minimize bullying in prison. Although there is research on the relationship between lack of empathy and positive attitude toward bullying, previous research has overlooked that age may influence this relationship. In fact, previous research has shown that there are changes in empathy across the life span. Therefore, we examined whether having a positive attitude toward bullying in offenders was predicted by age, mediated by cognitive/affective empathy. Another important factor in the prediction of positive attitudes toward bullying may be the belief in a just world, as having a weak belief is related to more aggressive outbursts. Given that there is scarce research in the topic, we examined the relationship between having a positive attitude toward bullying and personal belief in a just world. To that aim, 123 sentenced adult male prisoners, selected from a Category C prison in the United Kingdom completed different questionnaires to assess their levels of cognitive and affective empathy, positive attitude toward bullying, and personal belief in a just world. As expected, age predicted a positive attitude toward bullying, mediated by affective empathy. However, we did not find a positive relationship between a positive attitude toward bullying and a personal belief in a just world. The results are discussed in terms of their application in possible intervention programs.


Journal of Genetic Psychology | 2017

Studying Children's Intrapersonal Emotion Regulation Strategies from the Process Model of Emotion Regulation

Belén López-Pérez; Michaela Gummerum; Ellie L. Wilson; Giulia Dellaria

ABSTRACT The authors relied on the Process Model of Emotion Regulation (PMER; J. J. Gross, 2007) to investigate childrens abilities to regulate their emotions and to assess how distinct emotion regulation strategies are used by children of different ages. In Study 1, 180 parents of children aged between 3 and 8 years old reported about a situation in which their child had been able to change what she or he was feeling. In Study 2, 126 children 3–8 years old answered 2 questions about how they regulate their own emotions. Results from both studies showed age differences in childrens reported emotion regulation abilities and the strategies they used. As expected, strategies such as situation selection, situation modification, and cognitive change were used more frequently by 5–6- and 7–8-year-olds, whereas attention deployment was mainly used by 3–4-year-olds. No age differences were found for response modulation. The present research contributes to the existing body of literature on emotion regulation by adding more information about the developmental patterns for each specific emotion regulation strategy.


British Journal of Clinical Psychology | 2017

Interpersonal emotion regulation in Asperger's syndrome and borderline personality disorder

Belén López-Pérez; Tamara Ambrona; Michaela Gummerum

OBJECTIVESnInterpersonal emotion regulation (ER) plays a significant role in how individuals meet others emotional needs and shape social interactions, as it is key to initiating and maintaining high-quality social relationships. Given that individuals with borderline personality disorder (BPD) or Aspergers syndrome (AS) exhibit problems in social interactions, the aim of this study was to examine their use of different interpersonal ER strategies compared to normative control participants.nnnMETHODSnThirty individuals with AS, 30 with BPD, and 60 age-, gender-, and education-matched control participants completed a battery of measures to assess interpersonal ER, which assessed to what extent participants tended to engage in interpersonal affect improvement and worsening and to what extent they used different strategies. Before completing those measures, all groups were screened for disorders of Axis I and Axis II with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I and Axis II Disorders.nnnRESULTSnCompared to controls, individuals with AS and with BPD engaged less in affect improvement. No differences were found for affect worsening. Individuals with AS reported to use less adaptive (attention deployment, cognitive change) and more maladaptive (expressive suppression) interpersonal ER strategies, compared to individuals with BPD and control participants who did not differ from each other.nnnCONCLUSIONSnThe obtained results suggest the need to develop tailored ER interventions for each of the clinical groups studied. Furthermore, they highlight the need to study further potential differences in intrapersonal and interpersonal ER in clinical populations.nnnPRACTITIONER POINTSnIndividuals with Aspergers syndrome (AS) and borderline personality disorder (BPD) engaged significantly less than healthy controls in interpersonal affect improvement. Individuals with BPD did not differ from healthy controls in the use of interpersonal strategies. Individuals with AS reported to use more maladaptive and less adaptive strategies than BPD individuals and healthy controls. Understanding differences in interpersonal emotion regulation in individuals with AS and with BPD and normative controls might help practitioners develop better interventions.


PLOS ONE | 2018

Prospective mental imagery as its link with anxiety and depression in prisoners.

Belén López-Pérez; Catherine Deeprose; Yaniv Hanoch

Mental imagery is known to play a key role in the development and maintenance of depression and anxiety. Prisoners commonly experience psychological distress, but interventions to address this are currently lacking. We aimed to examine the link between prospective mental imagery and anxiety and depression among prisoners. One hundred twenty-three male prisoners from a Category C prison in southwest England participated in the study. They completed the Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) and the General Anxiety Disorder Scale (GAD-7) to measure whether they experience depression and/or anxiety symptoms. Furthermore, they completed additional questionnaires to evaluate their prospective mental imagery. Results showed that 67.5% of prisoners presented with more depression symptoms and 27.7% with more anxiety symptoms. Supporting earlier findings, our data revealed that some dimensions of prospective mental imagery were significantly related with increased anxiety and depression symptoms in prisoners. Namely, intrusive negative personally relevant imagery was a positive predictor and likelihood of positive events a negative predictor of both anxiety and depression symptoms. The perceived likelihood of negative events was a positive predictor of depression. Intrusive verbal thought was a positive predictor of anxiety. The obtained results suggest the need to develop interventions not only targeting the reduction of prospective negative imagery but also the enhancement of positive mental imagery.


PLOS ONE | 2018

Correction: Prospective mental imagery as its link with anxiety and depression in prisoners

Belén López-Pérez; Catherine Deeprose; Yaniv Hanoch

[This corrects the article DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0191551.].

Collaboration


Dive into the Belén López-Pérez's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ellie L. Wilson

Plymouth State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Yaniv Hanoch

Plymouth State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Luis Oceja

Autonomous University of Madrid

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Eric L. Stocks

University of Texas at Tyler

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Giulia Dellaria

Plymouth State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Janice Sánchez

University College London

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Sergio Salgado

University of La Frontera

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge