Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Eric L. Stocks is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Eric L. Stocks.


Evolutionary Psychology | 2008

Are Sexual and Emotional Infidelity Equally Upsetting to Men and Women? Making Sense of Forced-Choice Responses

David A. Lishner; Shannon Nguyen; Eric L. Stocks; Emily J. Zillmer

Forced-choice measures that assess reactions to imagined sexual and emotional infidelity are ubiquitous in studies testing the Jealousy as a Specific Innate Module (JSIM) model. One potential problem with such measures is that they fail to identify respondents who find both forms of infidelity equally upsetting. To examine this issue, an experiment was conducted in which two groups of participants imagined a romantic infidelity after which participants in the first group used a traditional forced-choice measure to indicate whether they found sexual or emotional infidelity more upsetting. Participants in the second group instead used a modified forced-choice measure that allowed them also to indicate whether they found both forms of infidelity equally upsetting. Consistent with previous research, those given the traditional forced-choice measure tended to respond in a manner that supported the JSIM model. However, the majority of participants given the modified measure indicated that both forms of infidelity were equally upsetting.


Basic and Applied Social Psychology | 2014

Empathy, awareness of others, and action: How feeling empathy for one-among-others motivates helping the others

Luis Oceja; Marc W. Heerdink; Eric L. Stocks; Tamara Ambrona; Belén López-Pérez; Sergio Salgado

Feeling empathy for a member of the group may result in either favoring this individual at the expense of the group or helping the entire group. We explain these intriguing findings by proposing that the combined influence of feeling empathy for one individual and awareness of others enhances willingness to help both the individual and the others (taken as individuals). The results of three experiments showed that inducing empathy for one individual promotes favoring him or her at the expense of the group, whereas inducing empathy for one-among-others leads to helping these others individually, instead of as a group. Furthermore, the awareness of others mediated the proposed one-among-others effect.


Nurse Education Today | 2013

Feeling at hospitals: Perspective-taking, empathy and personal distress among professional nurses and nursing students

Belén López-Pérez; Tamara Ambrona; Jennifer Gregory; Eric L. Stocks; Luis Oceja

When facing a person in need, professional nurses will tend to adopt an objective perspective compared to nursing students who, instead, will tend to adopt an imagine-other perspective. Consequently, professional nurses will show lower vicarious emotional reaction such as empathy and distress. Using samples from Spain (Studies 1 and 2) and United states (Study 3), we compared perspective taking strategies and the emotional responses of nurses and nursing students when perceiving a sick child (Study 1) and a sick adult (Studies 2 and 3). Taken together, the results supported our hypotheses. We discuss the applied value of considering the relationship between perspective-taking and its emotional consequences for the nursing profession.


The Journal of Psychology | 2018

Does Feeling Empathy Lead to Compassion Fatigue or Compassion Satisfaction? The Role of Time Perspective

Eric M. Hansen; Jakob Eklund; Anna Hallén; Carmen Stockman Bjurhager; Emil Norrström; Adam Viman; Eric L. Stocks

Abstract Research has shown that feeling empathy sometimes leads to compassion fatigue and sometimes to compassion satisfaction. In three studies, participants recalled an instance when they felt empathy in order to assess the role time perspective plays in how empathizers perceive the consequences of empathy. Study 1 revealed that college students perceive empathy as having more negative consequences in the short term, but more positive consequences in the long term. Study 2 showed that service industry professionals perceive the consequences of feeling empathy for customers who felt bad as less negative, and the consequences of feeling empathy for people who felt good as less positive, in the long as opposed to the short term. Because Studies 1 and 2 confounded time perspective with event specificity a third study was conducted in which event specificity was held constant across time perspectives. The same pattern of results emerged. The results of these studies indicate that perceptions of the effects of feeling empathy, whether positive or negative, become less extreme over time. These findings shed light on the relation between empathy and compassion fatigue and satisfaction by suggesting that situations that initially are experienced as stressful can over time make the empathizer stronger.


International Journal of Psychology | 2018

How is your day going? Reciprocity Norm in Everyday Communication

Eric L. Stocks; Felicia Mirghassemi; Luis Oceja

In 6 experiments, we manipulated the length of a communication message to assess reciprocity norm-following behaviour during conversational interaction. In Studies 1 and 2, scripts of different length were used in an online chat, and chat logs were analysed. In Studies 3 and 4, participants e-mailed scripts that varied in length to acquaintances, and replies were analysed. In Studies 5 and 6, a confederate initiated conversations with either strangers or acquaintances and recorded the amount of time the person responded. The results suggest that conversation partners reciprocate length across different communication media, regardless of the depth of the information disclosed.


Intergroup helping, 2017, ISBN 9783319530246, págs. 183-204 | 2017

The Path from Helping One to Helping the Group and Beyond

Luis Oceja; Eric L. Stocks

This chapter presents two lines of research about two motives that may lead people to helping a group other than one’s own. First, our research on the one-among-others effect posits that inducing empathic concern for a victim who is presented along with other individuals in need may enhance the willingness to increase the welfare of others (i.e. generalised altruism). Second, our research on the world-change orientation posits the existence of a social motive, quixoteism, with the ultimate goal of increasing the welfare of the world. The proximal processes that may elicit these motives, and their influence on the decision and maintenance of intergroup helping contexts, are discussed.


European Journal of Social Psychology | 2009

Altruism or psychological escape: Why does empathy promote prosocial behavior?†

Eric L. Stocks; David A. Lishner; Stephanie K. Decker


Journal of Applied Social Psychology | 2011

I'm Embarrassed for You: The Effect of Valuing and Perspective Taking on Empathic Embarrassment and Empathic Concern

Eric L. Stocks; David A. Lishner; Bethany L. Waits; Eirah M. Downum


Motivation and Emotion | 2008

The effect of infant-like characteristics on empathic concern for adults in need

David A. Lishner; Luis Oceja; Eric L. Stocks; Kirstin Zaspel


Archive | 2011

Four forms of prosocial motivation egoism, altruism, collectivism, and principlism

C. Daniel Batson; Nadia Ahmad; Eric L. Stocks

Collaboration


Dive into the Eric L. Stocks's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Luis Oceja

Autonomous University of Madrid

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

David A. Lishner

University of Wisconsin–Oshkosh

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Pilar Carrera

Autonomous University of Madrid

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Felicia Mirghassemi

University of Texas at Tyler

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Bethany L. Waits

University of Texas at Tyler

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge