Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Bruno Fimm is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Bruno Fimm.


Neuropsychologia | 2004

Network for auditory intrinsic alertness: a PET study.

Walter Sturm; Francesca Longoni; Bruno Fimm; Thomas Dietrich; Susanne Weis; Stefan Kemna; Hans Herzog; Klaus Willmes

Intrinsic alertness designates the internal (cognitive) control of wakefulness and arousal; typical tasks to assess optimal levels of intrinsic alertness are simple reaction time (RT) measurements without preceding warning stimuli. Until now, cerebral networks subserving alertness after visual and somatosensory stimulation have been reported. Studies concerning other intensity aspects of alertness like sustained attention and vigilance, on the other hand, have been performed in the auditory modality, too. In a 15O-butanol PET-activation study in 10 right-handed young healthy male volunteers an intrinsic alertness network was studied for the auditory modality. In contrast with a sensorimotor control condition we found an extended predominantly right-hemisphere network similar to those reported for other sensory modalities including frontal, cingular, inferior parietal, temporal and thalamic structures, when subjects waited for and rapidly responded to a 1000 Hz tone signal by pressing a response key with the right-hand thumb. There were, however, some differences in the topography of the frontal, temporal and thalamic activations between auditory and visual stimulation which are discussed with respect to similar results for auditory vigilance and auditory selective attention tasks reported in the literature.


Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking | 2008

Depth Perception in Virtual Reality: Distance Estimations in Peri- and Extrapersonal Space

Claudia Armbrüster; Marc Wolter; Torsten W. Kuhlen; Wilhelmus Spijkers; Bruno Fimm

The present study investigated depth perception in virtual environments. Twenty-three participants verbally estimated ten distances between 40 cm and 500 cm in three different virtual environments in two conditions: (1) only one target was presented or (2) ten targets were presented at the same time. Additionally, the presence of a metric aid was varied. A questionnaire assessed subjective ratings about physical complaints (e.g., headache), the experience in the virtual world (e.g., presence), and the experiment itself (self-evaluation of the estimations). Results show that participants underestimate the virtual distances but are able to perceive the distances in the right metric order even when only very simple virtual environments are presented. Furthermore, interindividual differences and intraindividual stabilities can be found among participants, and neither the three different virtual environments nor the metric aid improved depth estimations. Estimation performance is better in peripersonal than in extrapersonal space. In contrast, subjective ratings provide a preferred space: a closed room with visible floor, ceiling, and walls.


Cognitive Brain Research | 2003

Cerebral activation in abstinent ecstasy (MDMA) users during a working memory task: a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study

Jörg Daumann; Bruno Fimm; Klaus Willmes; Armin Thron; Euphrosyne Gouzoulis-Mayfrank

The popular recreational drug ecstasy (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine=MDMA and related congeners) is neurotoxic upon central serotonergic systems in animal studies. So far, the most convincing evidence for neurotoxicity-related functional deficits in humans derives from neurocognitive studies demonstrating dose-related long-term learning and memory problems in ecstasy users. In our study we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and a working memory task to investigate cerebral activation in eleven heavy, but currently abstinent MDMA users and two equally sized groups of moderate users and non-users. There were no significant group differences in working memory performance and no differences in cortical activation patterns for a conservative level of significance. However, for a more liberal statistical criterion, both user groups showed stronger activations than controls in right parietal cortex. Furthermore, heavy users had a weaker blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) response than moderate users and controls in frontal and temporal areas. Our results may indicate subtle altered brain functioning associated with prior MDMA use, although alternative interpretations of these group differences must be considered.


Neuropsychologia | 2006

The effect of low arousal on visuo-spatial attention

Bruno Fimm; Klaus Willmes; Will Spijkers

Clinical and experimental evidence suggests a special role of the right hemisphere for alerting and orienting. However, it is not clear whether these attentional aspects should be taken to be independent or interacting. At least on the functional neuroanatomical level there seems to be an overlap in right parietal and frontal cortical structures. In this sleep deprivation study we examined the effect of different levels of arousal on covert orienting of attention repeatedly over a period of 28 h in 11 healthy subjects. The main finding is a disproportionate and significant slowing of responses to stimuli presented to the left visual hemifield that could only be observed in a state of maximally reduced arousal at 5:00 a.m. Furthermore, a facilitation of attentional shifts to the right could be observed in invalid trials when attention had to be reoriented. These results suggest an interaction of arousal and orienting, at least in situations of maximally reduced activation when attentional asymmetries could be provoked even in healthy subjects. The findings are compatible with a right hemisphere dominance for alerting and orienting, and they are discussed in the light of theoretical accounts of visual orienting.


Experimental Brain Research | 2006

Spatial attention: more than intrinsic alerting?

Walter Sturm; B. Schmenk; Bruno Fimm; Karsten Specht; Susanne Weis; Armin Thron; Klaus Willmes

It has been proposed that the right hemisphere alerting network co-activates, either directly or via the brainstem, the attention system in the parietal cortex involved in spatial attention. The observation that impaired alertness and sustained attention can predict the outcome of neglect might suggest such a relationship, too. In the present fMRI study, we intended to analyse and compare the functional anatomy of two attentional conditions both involving intrinsic (endogenous) alerting and fixation but differing with respect to the degree of spatially distributed attention by using the same paradigm under two different attentional conditions. In a group of ten participants, both a focused and a distributed visuospatial attention condition evoked similar patterns of activation in dorsolateral prefrontal regions, in the anterior cingulate gyrus, in the superior and inferior parietal cortex as well as in the superior temporal gyrus and in the thalamus. These activation foci were stronger in the right hemisphere under both conditions. After subtraction of the alertness condition with focused spatial attention, distributed spatial attention with stimuli appearing at unpredictable locations within both visual fields induced additional bilateral activations only in the left and right superior parietal cortex and in the right precuneus suggesting that these regions are specific for a more widespread dispersion of spatial attention.


Human Brain Mapping | 2007

Management of attentional resources in within‐modal and cross‐modal divided attention tasks: An fMRI study

René Vohn; Bruno Fimm; Jochen Weber; Ralph Schnitker; Armin Thron; Will Spijkers; Klaus Willmes; Walter Sturm

In the present study, we were interested in distinguishing the cortical representations of within‐modal and cross‐modal divided attention tasks by using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Sixteen healthy male subjects aged between 21 and 30 years underwent two within‐modal (auditory/auditory, visual/visual) and one cross‐modal (auditory/visual) divided attention task, as well as related selective attention control conditions. After subtraction of the corresponding control task the three divided attention tasks, irrespective of sensory modality, revealed significant activation in a predominantly right hemisphere network involving the prefrontal cortex, the inferior parietal cortex, and the claustrum. Under the cross‐modal condition, however, the frontal and parietal activation was more extended and more bilateral and there also was stronger right hemisphere activation of the anterior cingulate cortex and the thalamus. In comparison to the within‐modal conditions additional bilateral frontal and left inferior parietal activation was found for the cross‐modal condition. The supplementary fronto‐parietal, anterior cingulate cortex, and thalamus activation in the auditory/visual condition could be argued to reflect an additional demand for coordination of two ongoing cross‐modal cognitive processes. Hum Brain Mapp, 2007.


Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders | 2012

Alternate-Form Reliability of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment Screening Test in a Clinical Setting

Ana S. Costa; Bruno Fimm; Paul Friesen; Herve Soundjock; Claudia Rottschy; Theresa Gross; Frank Eitner; Arno Reich; Jörg B. Schulz; Ziad Nasreddine; Kathrin Reetz

Aims: The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) has gained recognition for its validity in detecting cognitive impairment in several clinical populations. For serial assessments, alternate forms are needed to overcome possible practice effects. Our objective was to investigate the reliability of two German MoCA alternate forms for longitudinal assessment applications. Methods: The original and one of two alternate forms of the MoCA were administered within a 60-min interval of a clinical interview in a counterbalanced order to 100 healthy elderly controls, 30 patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and 30 patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD). The diagnosis of the majority of patients was supported by in vivo AD pathology biomarkers. Results: There was a strong correlation between the alternate forms and the original MoCA in all groups, but particularly in the clinical samples. Total mean scores did not differ significantly between the MoCA versions, even taking into account the presentation order. As in previous studies, age and education influenced performance in the MoCA. The same pattern of group differences (controls > MCI > AD) was observed for each of the versions. Conclusion: All three forms can be reliably and interchangeably used in serial cognitive assessment, confirming the MoCA’s applicability in research and clinical longitudinal approaches.


NeuroImage | 2006

A special role for numbers in working memory? An fMRI study

André Knops; Hans-Christoph Nuerk; Bruno Fimm; René Vohn; Klaus Willmes

Although numbers carry averbal semantics (i.e., magnitude), they are often utilized in verbal Working Memory (vWM) tasks. However, vWM is thought to rely on a purely phonological code. Here, we tested the influence of (a) averbal semantics and (b) different tasks on performance in a vWM context by examining stimulus and task-specific variation of activity in the horizontal parts of the intraparietal sulcus (hIPS). The hIPS has previously been shown to subserve magnitude processing modulated by (a) specific stimuli and (b) specific tasks. Two variants of an n-back paradigm (comparison and identity match tasks) utilizing letters and numbers as stimulus material at different levels of vWM load were administered in 16 participants. Behavioral and functional imaging data reveal stimulus-specific modulation of activity in the hIPS suggesting a semantic influence of numbers. In the identity match task, numbers induced additional hIPS activation compared to letters while letters never induced additional hIPS activation when compared to numbers. Letters as compared to numbers only induced additional hIPS activation in the comparison task. These results question the assumption of a purely phonological code in vWM because hIPS activation subserving magnitude processing is modulated by stimulus semantics and task demands.


Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology | 2010

Predictive validity of attentional functions in differentiating children with and without ADHD: a componential analysis.

Liane Kaufmann; Nikola Zieren; Sibylle Zotter; Daniela Karall; Sabine Scholl-Bürgi; Edda Haberlandt; Bruno Fimm

Aim  The objective of this study was to investigate which attentional components are of predictive utility in differentiating children with attention‐deficit–hyperactivity disorder, combined type (ADHD‐C) from their peers without ADHD.


Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry | 2001

Asymmetries of visual attention after circumscribed subcortical vascular lesions.

Bruno Fimm; Roland Zahn; M Mull; Stefan Kemeny; F Buchwald; F Block; Michael Schwarz

OBJECTIVE To investigate the role of the basal ganglia and the thalamus for basic processes of visuospatial attention METHODS Fifteen patients with acute circumscribed vascular lesions (10 with haemorrhage and five with infarction) were included in the study. The lesions were confined exclusively to subcortical structures, such as the basal ganglia, internal capsule, and thalamus, which was confirmed by initial CT on the day of referral and MRI taken 14–28 days after clinical onset. These patients were examined with two computerised attentional tasks (one detection and one search task) measuring spatial visual attention. RESULTS There was a clear attentional asymmetry in patients with right hemispheric lesions (RHLs) in the visual search task. Seven out of eight patients with RHLs tended to be slower and/or missed significantly more target stimuli in the left sided part of a stimulus array consisting of 25 small squares than in right sided parts, although none of these patients showed signs of visual hemineglect in the visual detection task presenting visual information simultaneously to the right and left visual hemispace. All but one of these patients showed lesions in the posterior limb of the internal capsule and the putamen. On the other hand, patients with left hemispheric lesions were not impaired in the search task with only one patient showing more contralesional omissions of target stimuli than could be expected from the behaviour of normal controls. CONCLUSIONS The results are in line with previous results showing a dominant role of right hemispheric neuronal structures for spatial attention. Furthermore, the data suggest that even with right hemispheric subcortical lesions without cortical involvement deficits in spatial orienting of attention to the left hemispace can be seen. These asymmetries of visual attention in the absence of neglect symptoms are supposed to be caused (1) by a disruption of the motor corticostriato-pallidothalamo-cortical neuronal circuit or (2) by a (partial) disconnection of relevant parts within the posterior attention network—namely, parietal and thalamic structures.

Collaboration


Dive into the Bruno Fimm's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Marc Wolter

RWTH Aachen University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

René Vohn

RWTH Aachen University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Armin Thron

RWTH Aachen University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge