Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Catriona M.M. Lakemond is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Catriona M.M. Lakemond.


Food Chemistry | 2013

Extraction and characterisation of protein fractions from five insect species

Liya Yi; Catriona M.M. Lakemond; L.M.C. Sagis; Verena Eisner-Schadler; Arnold van Huis; Martinus A.J.S. van Boekel

Tenebrio molitor, Zophobas morio, Alphitobius diaperinus, Acheta domesticus and Blaptica dubia were evaluated for their potential as a future protein source. Crude protein content ranged from 19% to 22% (Dumas analysis). Essential amino acid levels in all insect species were comparable with soybean proteins, but lower than for casein. After aqueous extraction, next to a fat fraction, a supernatant, pellet, and residue were obtained, containing 17-23%, 33-39%, 31-47% of total protein, respectively. At 3% (w/v), supernatant fractions did not form stable foams and gels at pH 3, 5, 7, and 10, except for gelation for A. domesticus at pH 7. At 30% w/v, gels at pH 7 and pH 10 were formed, but not at pH 3 and pH 5. In conclusion, the insect species studied have potential to be used in foods due to: (1) absolute protein levels; (2) protein quality; (3) ability to form gels.


Food Hydrocolloids | 2003

Gelation of soy glycinin; influence of pH and ionic strength on network structure in relation to protein conformation

Catriona M.M. Lakemond; Harmen H. J. de Jongh; Marcel Paques; Ton van Vliet; Harry Gruppen; A.G.J. Voragen

Abstract Formation and structure of glycinin gels were studied in relation to protein conformation for two pH values and three ionic strengths. While at I =0.03 the gels were found to be fine stranded, gel coarseness increased when the ionic strength was higher. At I =0.03 finer gel network structures were formed at pH 3.8 than at pH 7.6, whereas for I =0.2 and 0.5 the reverse was found. The observed differences in gel stiffness (rheological dynamical measurements) did not correspond to coarseness of the gels. It was found that the nature of the primary network particles was different at pH 7.6 compared to pH 3.8, since at pH 7.6 only 51–69% of total protein was incorporated in the gel network (predominantly basic polypeptides), while at pH 3.8 all protein was present in the network. The higher water holding capacities observed at pH 7.6 compared to pH 3.8 support the idea that at pH 7.6 the non-network protein resides in the pores. At all conditions studied denaturation coincides with the induction of β-sheet at a secondary level (IR measurements), and with gel formation (except for I =0.03). The largest increase in gel stiffness did not take place directly after denaturation but during the cooling part of the temperature cycle used. This increase in gel stiffness could not be related to changes in secondary structure.


Journal of Agricultural and Food Chemistry | 2017

Nitrogen-to-Protein Conversion Factors for Three Edible Insects: Tenebrio molitor, Alphitobius diaperinus, and Hermetia illucens

Renske H. Janssen; Jean Paul Vincken; Lambertus A.M. van den Broek; Vincenzo Fogliano; Catriona M.M. Lakemond

Insects are considered a nutritionally valuable source of alternative proteins, and their efficient protein extraction is a prerequisite for large-scale use. The protein content is usually calculated from total nitrogen using the nitrogen-to-protein conversion factor (Kp) of 6.25. This factor overestimates the protein content, due to the presence of nonprotein nitrogen in insects. In this paper, a specific Kp of 4.76 ± 0.09 was calculated for larvae from Tenebrio molitor, Alphitobius diaperinus, and Hermetia illucens, using amino acid analysis. After protein extraction and purification, a Kp factor of 5.60 ± 0.39 was found for the larvae of three insect species studied. We propose to adopt these Kp values for determining protein content of insects to avoid overestimation of the protein content.


Appetite | 2012

Effect of salt intensity in soup on ad libitum intake and on subsequent food choice

Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis; Catriona M.M. Lakemond; René A. de Wijk; P.A. Luning; Cees de Graaf

The effect of salt intensity on ad libitum intake of tomato soup was investigated when soup was served as a first course and as a second course. Also the effect of salt intensity in soup on subsequent sweet vs. savory choice of sandwich fillings was investigated. Forty-three healthy subjects consumed ad libitum a low-salt (LS), ideal-salt (IS) and high-salt (HS) tomato soup in both meal settings. The salt concentrations were selected on an individual basis, in a way that IS was most pleasant and LS and HS were similar in pleasantness. The ad libitum intake of IS soup was higher than that of LS and HS soup, and the ad libitum intake of LS soup was higher than that of HS soup. The meal setting, soup as a first or as a second course, did not affect ad libitum intake. Salt intensity in soup did not predict sweet vs. savory choice of fillings in grams or energy, although most sodium from fillings was consumed after intake of HS soup. In conclusion, a higher salt intensity lead to lower ad libitum intake of soup similar in palatability (LS vs. HS). In addition, salt intensity in soup does not predict sweet vs. savory food choice.


Chemical Senses | 2010

Effect of Salt Intensity on Ad Libitum Intake of Tomato Soup Similar in Palatability and on Salt Preference after Consumption

Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis; Catriona M.M. Lakemond; René A. de Wijk; P.A. Luning; Cees de Graaf

Sensory properties of food play an important role in satiation. Studies on the effect of taste intensity on satiation show conflicting results. This may be due to the notion that in these studies taste intensity and palatability were confounded. The objective of this study was to investigate the effect of salt intensity of tomato soup on ad libitum intake (satiation), while controlling for palatability on an individual basis. Forty-eight subjects consumed both a low-salt (LS) and high-salt (HS) soup ad libitum from a self-refilling bowl. The results showed no difference between LS and HS soup in ad libitum intake, eating rate, changes in appetite ratings, and changes in hedonic ratings after intake. After intake of HS soup, LS soup was perceived as more bland than before intake of HS soup. After intake of LS soup, HS soup was perceived as more salt intense than before intake of LS soup. In conclusion, this study found no effect of salt intensity on satiation of tomato soups that were similar in palatability. During consumption, subjects adapted quickly to the exposed salt intensity as contrasting salt intensities were rated further from the ideal salt intensity and therefore perceived as less pleasant after consumption.


International Journal of Obesity | 2014

Odors: appetizing or satiating? Development of appetite during odor exposure over time.

M.G. Ramaekers; Sanne Boesveldt; Catriona M.M. Lakemond; M A J S van Boekel; P.A. Luning

Background:Exposure to palatable food odors influences appetite responses, either promoting or inhibiting food intake. Possibly, food odors are appetizing after a short exposure (of circa 1–3 min), but become satiating over time (circa 10–20 min).Objective:To investigate the effect of odor exposure on general appetite and sensory-specific appetite (SSA) over time.Design:In a cross-over study, 21 unrestrained women (age: 18–45 years; BMI: 18.5–25 kg m−2) were exposed for 20 min to eight different odor types: five food odors, two nonfood odors and no-odor. All odors were distributed in a test room at suprathreshold levels. General appetite, SSA and salivation were measured over time.Results:All food odors significantly increased general appetite and SSA, compared with the no-odor condition. The nonfood odors decreased general appetite. All effects did not change over time during odor exposure. Savory odors increased the appetite for savory foods, but decreased appetite for sweet foods, and vice versa after exposure to sweet odors. Neither food odors nor nonfood odors affected salivation.Conclusions:Palatable food odors were appetizing during and after odor exposure and did not become satiating over a 20-min period. Food odors had a large impact on SSA and a small impact on general appetite. Moreover, exposure to food odors increased the appetite for congruent foods, but decreased the appetite for incongruent foods. It may be hypothesized that, once the body is prepared for intake of a certain food with a particular macronutrient composition, it is unfavorable to consume foods that are very different from the cued food.


PLOS ONE | 2013

Consumption with Large Sip Sizes Increases Food Intake and Leads to Underestimation of the Amount Consumed

Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis; Catriona M.M. Lakemond; René A. de Wijk; P.A. Luning; Cees de Graaf

Background A number of studies have shown that bite and sip sizes influence the amount of food intake. Consuming with small sips instead of large sips means relatively more sips for the same amount of food to be consumed; people may believe that intake is higher which leads to faster satiation. This effect may be disturbed when people are distracted. Objective The objective of the study is to assess the effects of sip size in a focused state and a distracted state on ad libitum intake and on the estimated amount consumed. Design In this 3×2 cross-over design, 53 healthy subjects consumed ad libitum soup with small sips (5 g, 60 g/min), large sips (15 g, 60 g/min), and free sips (where sip size was determined by subjects themselves), in both a distracted and focused state. Sips were administered via a pump. There were no visual cues toward consumption. Subjects then estimated how much they had consumed by filling soup in soup bowls. Results Intake in the small-sip condition was ∼30% lower than in both the large-sip and free-sip conditions (P<0.001). In addition, subjects underestimated how much they had consumed in the large-sip and free-sip conditions (P<0.03). Distraction led to a general increase in food intake (P = 0.003), independent of sip size. Distraction did not influence sip size or estimations. Conclusions Consumption with large sips led to higher food intake, as expected. Large sips, that were either fixed or chosen by subjects themselves led to underestimations of the amount consumed. This may be a risk factor for over-consumption. Reducing sip or bite sizes may successfully lower food intake, even in a distracted state.


British Journal of Nutrition | 2014

Aroma exposure time and aroma concentration in relation to satiation

M.G. Ramaekers; P.A. Luning; Rianne M.A.J. Ruijschop; Catriona M.M. Lakemond; Johannes H.F. Bult; Gerrit Gort; Martinus A.J.S. van Boekel

The present study investigated the effect of aroma exposure time and aroma concentration on ad libitum intake and subjective satiation. In a within-subject study, thirty-eight unrestrained, healthy female participants (age: 18-39 years; BMI: 18·5-26·0 kg/m²) were asked to consume tomato soup during lunchtime, until they felt comfortably full. Every 30 s, the participants consumed 10 g of a bland soup base while tomato soup aroma was delivered separately through the nose via a retronasal tube that was attached to an olfactometer. This gave the impression of consuming real tomato soup. For each sip, the aroma varied in exposure time (3 and 18 s) and concentration (5 × ), resulting in four different test conditions. Ad libitum food intake and appetite profile parameters were measured. A 9% lower food intake was observed when the participants were exposed to the condition with 18 s exposure time and a high concentration than when exposed to the other three conditions. These results indicate that changing the retronasal aroma release by aroma concentration and aroma exposure time affects food intake.


Journal of Nutrition | 2014

Sensory-Specific Appetite Is Affected by Actively Smelled Food Odors and Remains Stable Over Time in Normal-Weight Women

M.G. Ramaekers; Sanne Boesveldt; Gerrit Gort; Catriona M.M. Lakemond; Martinus A.J.S. van Boekel; P.A. Luning

Understanding overconsumption starts with knowledge of how separate factors influence our eating behavior. Food cues such as food odors are known for their effect on general appetite and sensory-specific appetite (SSA). Active sniffing rather than passive exposure may induce satiation over time. The objective of this study was to investigate how actively sniffing banana odors affects general appetite, SSA, and subsequent food intake. In a crossover study, 61 women actively smelled cups containing natural banana, artificial banana odor, or water (no odor) for 10 min. Treatment order was randomly assigned as much as possible. General appetite and SSA were monitored by using 100-mm visual analog scales during the 10 min of active sniffing, followed by ad libitum intake of banana milkshake. Results showed that SSA was consistently high (+12 mm) during actively sniffing natural or artificial banana odors, with no decrease in SSA over time. Sniffing both banana odors increased the appetite for banana (+11 mm) and other sweet products (+4 mm), whereas the appetite for savory products decreased by 7 mm (all P < 0.01) compared with no odor. Actively sniffing banana odor did not significantly influence food intake (P = 0.68) or general appetite scores (P = 0.06). In conclusion, SSA scores during active sniffing were identical to the SSA found in a similar study that used passive smelling, suggesting that SSA is independent of the manner of sniffing and exposure time. Moreover, sweet/savory categorization may suggest that food odors communicate information about the nutrient composition of their associated foods. These data clearly show the appetizing effects of food odors.


PLOS ONE | 2016

Food preference and appetite after switching between sweet and savoury odours in women

M.G. Ramaekers; P.A. Luning; Catriona M.M. Lakemond; Martinus A.J.S. van Boekel; Gerrit Gort; Sanne Boesveldt

Background Exposure to food odours increases the appetite for congruent foods and decreases the appetite for incongruent foods. However, the effect of exposure to a variety of food odours, as often occurs in daily life, is unknown. Objective Investigate how switching between sweet and savoury odours affects the appetite for sweet and savoury products. Design Thirty women (age: 18-45y; BMI: 18.5-25kg/m2) intensely smelled the contents of cups filled with banana, meat or water (no-odour) in a within-subject design with four combinations: no-odour/banana, no-odour/meat, meat/banana and banana/meat. Participants received one combination per test day. In each combination, two cups with different fillings were smelled for five minutes after each other. Treatment order was balanced as much as possible. The effects of previous exposure and current odour on the appetite for (in)congruent sweet and savoury products, and odour pleasantness were analysed. A change from meat to banana odour or banana to meat odour was referred to as switch, whereas a change from no-odour to meat odour or no-odour to banana odour was no-switch. Results The current odour (P<0.001), as opposed to the previous exposure (P = 0.71), determined the appetite for (in)congruent sweet and savoury products, already one minute after a switch between sweet and savoury odours. The pleasantness of the odour decreased during odour exposure (P = 0.005). Conclusions After a switch, the appetite for specific products quickly adjusted to the new odour and followed the typical pattern as found during odour exposure in previous studies. Interestingly, the appetite for the smelled food remained elevated during odour exposure, known as sensory-specific appetite, whereas the pleasantness of the odour decreased over time, previously termed olfactory sensory-specific satiety. This seeming contradiction may result from different mechanisms underlying the odour-induced anticipation of food intake versus the decrease in hedonic value during prolonged sensory stimulation.

Collaboration


Dive into the Catriona M.M. Lakemond's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

P.A. Luning

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Harry Gruppen

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

M.G. Ramaekers

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Martinus A.J.S. van Boekel

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Vincenzo Fogliano

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Cees de Graaf

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Liya Yi

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

René A. de Wijk

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ton van Vliet

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge