Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis
Wageningen University and Research Centre
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis.
Journal of Nutrition | 2011
Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis; Elisabeth H. M. Temme; Fari T. Koeman; Martijn W.J. Noort; Stefanie Kremer; A.M. Janssen
Bread is a major contributor to sodium intake in many countries. Reducing the salt (NaCl) content in bread might be an effective way to reduce overall sodium intake. The objectives of this study were to examine the effects of gradually lowering the salt content in brown bread, with and without flavor compensation (KCl and yeast extract), on bread consumption and sodium intake compensation by choice of sandwich fillings. A total of 116 participants (age: 21 ± 3 y; BMI: 22 ± 2 kg/m²) consumed a buffet-style breakfast on weekdays for 4 wk. Participants received either regular bread (control group: n = 39), bread whose salt content was gradually lowered each week by 0, 31, 52, and 67% (reduced group: n = 38), or bread whose salt content was also gradually lowered each week but which was also flavor compensated (compensated group: n = 39). A reduction of up to 52% of salt in bread did not lead to lower consumption of bread compared to the control (P = 0.57), whereas less bread was consumed when salt was reduced by 67% (P = 0.006). When bread was flavor compensated, however, a reduction of 67% did not lead to lower consumption (P = 0.69). Salt reduction in bread (with and without flavor compensation) did not induce sodium intake compensation (P = 0.31). In conclusion, a salt reduction of up to 52% in bread or even up to 67% in flavor-compensated bread neither affected bread consumption nor choice of sandwich fillings.
PLOS ONE | 2014
Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis; Ciarán G. Forde; Yuejiao Cheng; Haohuan Xu; Nathalie Martin; Cees de Graaf
Background Previous research has shown that oral processing characteristics like bite size and oral residence duration are related to the satiating efficiency of foods. Oral processing characteristics are influenced by food texture. Very little research has been done on the effect of food texture within solid foods on energy intake. Objectives The first objective was to investigate the effect of hardness of food on energy intake at lunch, and to link this effect to differences in food oral processing characteristics. The second objective was to investigate whether the reduction in energy intake at lunch will be compensated for in the subsequent dinner. Design Fifty subjects (11 male, BMI: 21±2 kg/m2, age: 24±2 y) participated in a cross-over study in which they consumed ad libitum from a lunch with soft foods or hard foods on two separate days. Oral processing characteristics at lunch were assessed by coding video records. Later on the same days, subjects consumed dinner ad libitum. Results Hard foods led to a ∼13% lower energy intake at lunch compared to soft foods (P<0.001). Hard foods were consumed with smaller bites, longer oral duration per gram food, and more chewing per gram food compared to the soft foods (P<0.05). Energy intake at dinner did not differ after both lunches (P = 0.16). Conclusions Hard foods led to reduced energy intake compared to soft foods, and this reduction in energy intake was sustained over the next meal. We argue that the differences in oral processing characteristics produced by the hardness of the foods explain the effect on intake. The sustained reduction in energy intake suggests that changes in food texture can be a helpful tool in reducing the overall daily energy intake.
Appetite | 2012
Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis; Catriona M.M. Lakemond; René A. de Wijk; P.A. Luning; Cees de Graaf
The effect of salt intensity on ad libitum intake of tomato soup was investigated when soup was served as a first course and as a second course. Also the effect of salt intensity in soup on subsequent sweet vs. savory choice of sandwich fillings was investigated. Forty-three healthy subjects consumed ad libitum a low-salt (LS), ideal-salt (IS) and high-salt (HS) tomato soup in both meal settings. The salt concentrations were selected on an individual basis, in a way that IS was most pleasant and LS and HS were similar in pleasantness. The ad libitum intake of IS soup was higher than that of LS and HS soup, and the ad libitum intake of LS soup was higher than that of HS soup. The meal setting, soup as a first or as a second course, did not affect ad libitum intake. Salt intensity in soup did not predict sweet vs. savory choice of fillings in grams or energy, although most sodium from fillings was consumed after intake of HS soup. In conclusion, a higher salt intensity lead to lower ad libitum intake of soup similar in palatability (LS vs. HS). In addition, salt intensity in soup does not predict sweet vs. savory food choice.
Chemical Senses | 2010
Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis; Catriona M.M. Lakemond; René A. de Wijk; P.A. Luning; Cees de Graaf
Sensory properties of food play an important role in satiation. Studies on the effect of taste intensity on satiation show conflicting results. This may be due to the notion that in these studies taste intensity and palatability were confounded. The objective of this study was to investigate the effect of salt intensity of tomato soup on ad libitum intake (satiation), while controlling for palatability on an individual basis. Forty-eight subjects consumed both a low-salt (LS) and high-salt (HS) soup ad libitum from a self-refilling bowl. The results showed no difference between LS and HS soup in ad libitum intake, eating rate, changes in appetite ratings, and changes in hedonic ratings after intake. After intake of HS soup, LS soup was perceived as more bland than before intake of HS soup. After intake of LS soup, HS soup was perceived as more salt intense than before intake of LS soup. In conclusion, this study found no effect of salt intensity on satiation of tomato soups that were similar in palatability. During consumption, subjects adapted quickly to the exposed salt intensity as contrasting salt intensities were rated further from the ideal salt intensity and therefore perceived as less pleasant after consumption.
PLOS ONE | 2013
Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis; Catriona M.M. Lakemond; René A. de Wijk; P.A. Luning; Cees de Graaf
Background A number of studies have shown that bite and sip sizes influence the amount of food intake. Consuming with small sips instead of large sips means relatively more sips for the same amount of food to be consumed; people may believe that intake is higher which leads to faster satiation. This effect may be disturbed when people are distracted. Objective The objective of the study is to assess the effects of sip size in a focused state and a distracted state on ad libitum intake and on the estimated amount consumed. Design In this 3×2 cross-over design, 53 healthy subjects consumed ad libitum soup with small sips (5 g, 60 g/min), large sips (15 g, 60 g/min), and free sips (where sip size was determined by subjects themselves), in both a distracted and focused state. Sips were administered via a pump. There were no visual cues toward consumption. Subjects then estimated how much they had consumed by filling soup in soup bowls. Results Intake in the small-sip condition was ∼30% lower than in both the large-sip and free-sip conditions (P<0.001). In addition, subjects underestimated how much they had consumed in the large-sip and free-sip conditions (P<0.03). Distraction led to a general increase in food intake (P = 0.003), independent of sip size. Distraction did not influence sip size or estimations. Conclusions Consumption with large sips led to higher food intake, as expected. Large sips, that were either fixed or chosen by subjects themselves led to underestimations of the amount consumed. This may be a risk factor for over-consumption. Reducing sip or bite sizes may successfully lower food intake, even in a distracted state.
Appetite | 2010
Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis; Catriona M.M. Lakemond; de R.A. Wijk; P.A. Luning; de C. Graaf
The aim of the present study was to investigate the effect of salt intensity on ad libitum intake of tomato soup. Secondly, the perceived salt intensity before and after ad libitum intake of tomato soups with varying saltiness concentrations was assessed. Subjects (n = 48) ate in duplo ad libitum from an individually determined low intense salt (LS) and high intense salt (HS) tomato soup using a self-refilling bowl. Before and after ad libitum intake, LS and HS soups were sampled and judged on salt intensity and pleasantness. The results showed no difference in intake between the two soups (LS 375 ± 172 g vs HS 388 ± 154 g). After intake of the LS soup, salt intensity for the HS soup was perceived as more intense salt and the pleasantness of HS soup decreased significantly more than the pleasantness of the just eaten LS soup (−22 ± 23 vs −9 ± 22 P < 0.05). After intake of the HS soup, the LS soup was perceived as less intense salt and the pleasantness of LS soup tended to be more decreased than the pleasantness of the just eaten HS soup (−20 ± 24 vs −14 ± 21 P = 0.2) In conclusion, we did not find an effect of salt intensity on ad libitum intake of tomato soup. Unexpectedly, the decrease in pleasantness for the ‘uneaten’ soup was more than for the just eaten soup, which is opposite to the “sensory specific satiety” effect. Subjects may obtain a new “reference point” of salt intensity after eaten a LS or HS soup. Therefore, they find the LS soup after intake of the HS soup too bland and the HS soup after intake of the LS soup much too salty. This could explain the higher decrease in pleasantness of the uneaten soup instead of the eaten soup.
Journal of Nutrition | 2011
Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis; Catriona M.M. Lakemond; René A. de Wijk; P.A. Luning; Cees de Graaf
Food Quality and Preference | 2014
Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis; Catriona M.M. Lakemond; René A. de Wijk; P.A. Luning; Cees de Graaf
Food Quality and Preference | 2012
V.A. Phan; M.G. Ramaekers; Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis; U. Garczarek; M.A.J.S. van Boekel; Matthijs Dekker
Appetite | 2013
Ciarán G. Forde; Dieuwerke P. Bolhuis; T. Thaler; C. de Graaf; Nathalie Martin