Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Christine Fawcett is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Christine Fawcett.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2010

Similarity predicts liking in 3-year-old children.

Christine Fawcett; Lori Markson

Two studies examined the influence of similarity on 3-year-old childrens initial liking of their peers. Children were presented with pairs of childlike puppets who were either similar or dissimilar to them on a specified dimension and then were asked to choose one of the puppets to play with as a measure of liking. Children selected the puppet whose food preferences or physical appearance matched their own. Unpacking the physical appearance finding revealed that the stable similarity of hair color may influence liking more strongly than the transient similarity of shirt color. A second study showed that children also prefer to play with a peer who shares their toy preferences, yet importantly, show no bias toward a peer who is similar on an arbitrary dimension. The findings provide insight into the earliest development of peer relations in young children.


PLOS ONE | 2014

The Child as Econometrician: A Rational Model of Preference Understanding in Children

Christopher G. Lucas; Thomas L. Griffiths; Fei Xu; Christine Fawcett; Alison Gopnik; Tamar Kushnir; Lori Markson; Jane Hu

Recent work has shown that young children can learn about preferences by observing the choices and emotional reactions of other people, but there is no unified account of how this learning occurs. We show that a rational model, built on ideas from economics and computer science, explains the behavior of children in several experiments, and offers new predictions as well. First, we demonstrate that when children use statistical information to learn about preferences, their inferences match the predictions of a simple econometric model. Next, we show that this same model can explain childrens ability to learn that other people have preferences similar to or different from their own and use that knowledge to reason about the desirability of hidden objects. Finally, we use the model to explain a developmental shift in preference understanding.


Psychological Science | 2016

Pupillary Contagion in Infancy Evidence for Spontaneous Transfer of Arousal

Christine Fawcett; Victoria Wesevich; Gustaf Gredebäck

Pupillary contagion—responding to pupil size observed in other people with changes in one’s own pupil—has been found in adults and suggests that arousal and other internal states could be transferred across individuals using a subtle physiological cue. Examining this phenomenon developmentally gives insight into its origins and underlying mechanisms, such as whether it is an automatic adaptation already present in infancy. In the current study, 6- and 9-month-olds viewed schematic depictions of eyes with smaller and larger pupils—pairs of concentric circles with smaller and larger black centers—while their own pupil sizes were recorded. Control stimuli were comparable squares. For both age groups, infants’ pupil size was greater when they viewed large-center circles than when they viewed small-center circles, and no differences were found for large-center compared with small-center squares. The findings suggest that infants are sensitive and responsive to subtle cues to other people’s internal states, a mechanism that would be beneficial for early social development.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2014

Infants' expectations about gestures and actions in third-party interactions.

Gudmundur Thorgrimsson; Christine Fawcett; Ulf Liszkowski

We investigated 14-month-old infants’ expectations toward a third party addressee of communicative gestures and an instrumental action. Infants’ eye movements were tracked as they observed a person (the Gesturer) point, direct a palm-up request gesture, or reach toward an object, and another person (the Addressee) respond by grasping it. Infants’ looking patterns indicate that when the Gesturer pointed or used the palm-up request, infants anticipated that the Addressee would give the object to the Gesturer, suggesting that they ascribed a motive of request to the gestures. In contrast, when the Gesturer reached for the object, and in a control condition where no action took place, the infants did not anticipate the Addressee’s response. The results demonstrate that infants’ recognition of communicative gestures extends to others’ interactions, and that infants can anticipate how third-party addressees will respond to others’ gestures.


Scientific Reports | 2017

Human eyes with dilated pupils induce pupillary contagion in infants

Christine Fawcett; Melda Arslan; Terje Falck-Ytter; Herbert Roeyers; Gustaf Gredebäck

Being sensitive and responsive to others’ internal states is critical for social life. One reliable cue to what others might be feeling is pupil dilation because it is linked to increases in arousal. When adults view an individual with dilated pupils, their pupils dilate in response, suggesting not only sensitivity to pupil size, but a corresponding response as well. However, little is known about the origins or mechanism underlying this phenomenon of pupillary contagion. Here we show that 4- to 6-month-old infants show pupillary contagion when viewing photographs of eyes with varying pupil sizes: their pupils dilate in response to others’ large, but not small or medium pupils. The results suggest that pupillary contagion is likely driven by a transfer of arousal and that it is present very early in life in human infants, supporting the view that it could be an adaptation fundamental for social and emotional development.


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry | 2016

Callous–unemotional traits affect adolescents' perception of collaboration

Christine Fawcett; Victoria Wesevich; Erik Truedsson; Cecilia Wåhlstedt; Gustaf Gredebäck

Background How is the perception of collaboration influenced by individual characteristics, in particular high levels of callous–unemotional (CU) traits? CU traits are associated with low empathy and endorsement of negative social goals such as dominance and forced respect. Thus, it is possible that they could relate to difficulties in interpreting that others are collaborating based on a shared goal. Methods In the current study, a community sample of 15‐ to 16‐year olds participated in an eye tracking task measuring whether they expect that others engaged in an action sequence are collaborating, depending on the emotion they display toward each other. Positive emotion would indicate that they share a goal, while negative emotion would indicate that they hold individual goals. Results When the actors showed positive emotion toward each other, expectations of collaboration varied with CU traits. The higher adolescents were on CU traits, the less likely they were to expect collaboration. When the actors showed negative emotion toward each other, CU traits did not influence expectations of collaboration. Conclusions The findings suggest that CU traits are associated with difficulty in perceiving positive social interactions, which could further contribute to the behavioral and emotional problems common to those with high CU traits.


Scientific Reports | 2018

Author Correction: Human eyes with dilated pupils induce pupillary contagion in infants

Christine Fawcett; Melda Arslan; Terje Falck-Ytter; Herbert Roeyers; Gustaf Gredebäck

A correction to this article has been published and is linked from the HTML and PDF versions of this paper. The error has not been fixed in the paper.


Developmental Psychology | 2010

Children reason about shared preferences

Christine Fawcett; Lori Markson


Infant and Child Development | 2012

Infants Anticipate Others' Social Preferences

Christine Fawcett; Ulf Liszkowski


Developmental Science | 2013

Infants use social context to bind actions into a collaborative sequence

Christine Fawcett; Gustaf Gredebäck

Collaboration


Dive into the Christine Fawcett's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Lori Markson

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Victoria Wesevich

Washington University in St. Louis

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Fei Xu

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge