Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Dana C. Torpey is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Dana C. Torpey.


Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology | 2011

Attentional biases for emotional faces in young children of mothers with chronic or recurrent depression.

Autumn Kujawa; Dana C. Torpey; Jiyon Kim; Greg Hajcak; Suzanne Rose; Ian H. Gotlib; Daniel N. Klein

Attentional biases for negative stimuli have been observed in school-age and adolescent children of depressed mothers and may reflect a vulnerability to depression. The direction of these biases and whether they can be identified in early childhood remains unclear. The current study examined attentional biases in 5–7-year-old children of depressed and non-depressed mothers. Following a mood induction, children participated in a dot-probe task assessing biases for sad and happy faces. There was a significant interaction of group and sex: daughters of depressed mothers attended selectively to sad faces, while children of controls and sons of depressed mothers did not exhibit biases. No effects were found for happy stimuli. These findings suggest that attentional biases are discernible in early childhood and may be vulnerability markers for depression. The results also raise the possibility that sex differences in cognitive biases are evident before the emergence of sex differences in the prevalence of depression.


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry | 2012

Electrocortical reactivity to emotional faces in young children and associations with maternal and paternal depression

Autumn Kujawa; Greg Hajcak; Dana C. Torpey; Jiyon Kim; Daniel N. Klein

BACKGROUND The late positive potential (LPP) is an event-related potential component that indexes selective attention toward motivationally salient information and is sensitive to emotional stimuli. Few studies have examined the LPP in children. Depression has been associated with reduced reactivity to negative and positive emotional stimuli, including reduced LPPs in response to emotional faces. The current study sought to identify the time course and scalp distribution of the LPP in response to emotional faces in young children and to determine whether reduced reactivity is observed among children at risk for depression. METHODS Electrocortical reactivity to emotional faces was examined in a large sample of young children and as a function of maternal and paternal depression. RESULTS In the overall sample, emotional faces were associated with increased positivities compared to neutral faces at occipital sites 200-600 ms after stimulus onset and at parietal sites 600-1,000 ms after stimulus onset. Children of mothers with a history of depressive disorders exhibited reduced differentiation in the early occipital LPP for emotional compared to neutral faces. CONCLUSIONS Results suggest that children as young as 6 years exhibit LPPs to emotional faces, and patterns of electrocortical reactivity to emotional stimuli may be associated with vulnerability to depressive disorders.


Developmental Psychobiology | 2012

Electrocortical and Behavioral Measures of Response Monitoring in Young Children During a Go/No-Go Task

Dana C. Torpey; Greg Hajcak; Jiyon Kim; Autumn Kujawa; Daniel N. Klein

The current study examined behavioral measures and response-locked event-related brain potentials (ERPs) derived from a Go/No-Go task in a large (N = 328) sample of 5- to 7-year-olds in order to better understand the early development of response monitoring and the impact of child age and sex. In particular, the error-related negativity (ERN, defined on both error trials alone and the difference between error and correct trials, or ΔERN), correct response negativity (CRN), and error positivity (P(e)) were examined. Overall, the ERN, CRN, and the P(e) were spatially and temporally similar to those measured in adults and older children. Even within our narrow age range, older children were faster and more accurate; a more negative ΔERN and a more positive P(e) were associated with: increasing age, increased accuracy, and faster reaction times on errors, suggesting these enhanced components reflected more efficient response monitoring of errors over development. Girls were slower and more accurate than boys, although both genders exhibited comparable ERPs. Younger children and girls were characterized by increased posterror slowing, although they did not demonstrate improved posterror accuracy. Posterror slowing was also related to a larger P(e) and reduced posterror accuracy. Collectively, these data suggest that posterror slowing may be unrelated to cognitive control and may, like the P(e), reflect an orienting response to errors.


Developmental Neuropsychology | 2009

An examination of error-related brain activity and its modulation by error value in young children.

Dana C. Torpey; Greg Hajcak; Daniel N. Klein

The error-related negativity (ERN) is an event-related brain potential observed in adults when errors are committed, and which appears to be sensitive to error value. Recent work suggests that the ERN can also be elicited in relatively young children using simple tasks and that ERN amplitude might be sensitive to error value. The current study employed a Go No-Go paradigm in which 5–7-year-old children (N = 18) earned low or high points for correct responses. Results indicated that errors were associated with an ERN; however, the size was not reliably moderated by error value.


Development and Psychopathology | 2011

Do positive and negative temperament traits interact in predicting risk for depression? A resting EEG study of 329 preschoolers

Stewart A. Shankman; Daniel N. Klein; Dana C. Torpey; Thomas M. Olino; Margaret W. Dyson; Jiyon Kim; C. Emily Durbin; Brady D. Nelson; Craig E. Tenke

Researchers have long been interested in whether particular temperamental traits in childhood connote risk for depressive disorders. For example, children characterized as having high negative emotionality (NE; sadness, fear, anger) and low positive emotionality (PE; anhedonia, listlessness, and lack of enthusiasm) are hypothesized to be at risk for depression. Few studies, however, have examined whether (and how) these two temperamental dimensions interact to confer risk. In a sample of 329 preschoolers, the present study addressed this question by examining the relation between PE and NE and asymmetry in resting EEG activity in frontal and posterior regions, which are putative biomarkers for depression. Using a laboratory battery to define temperament, we found an interaction of PE and NE on posterior asymmetry. Specifically, when PE was high, NE was associated with greater relative right activity. When PE was low, NE was not related to posterior asymmetry. These results were driven by differences in EEG activity in right posterior regions, an area associated with emotional processing and arousal, and were specific to girls. We found no relation between temperament and frontal asymmetry. These findings suggest that, at least for girls, PE and NE may have an interactive effect on risk for depression.


Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology | 2015

Erratum to: Self-reported and observed punitive parenting prospectively predicts increased error-related brain activity in six-year-old children

Alexandria Meyer; Greg Hajcak Proudfit; Sara J. Bufferd; Autumn Kujawa; Rebecca S. Laptook; Dana C. Torpey; Daniel N. Klein

The error-related negativity (ERN) is a negative deflection in the event-related potential (ERP) occurring approximately 50 ms after error commission at fronto-central electrode sites and is thought to reflect the activation of a generic error monitoring system. Several studies have reported an increased ERN in clinically anxious children, and suggest that anxious children are more sensitive to error commission—although the mechanisms underlying this association are not clear. We have previously found that punishing errors results in a larger ERN, an effect that persists after punishment ends. It is possible that learning-related experiences that impact sensitivity to errors may lead to an increased ERN. In particular, punitive parenting might sensitize children to errors and increase their ERN. We tested this possibility in the current study by prospectively examining the relationship between parenting style during early childhood and children’s ERN approximately 3 years later. Initially, 295 parents and children (approximately 3 years old) participated in a structured observational measure of parenting behavior, and parents completed a self-report measure of parenting style. At a follow-up assessment approximately 3 years later, the ERN was elicited during a Go/No-Go task, and diagnostic interviews were completed with parents to assess child psychopathology. Results suggested that both observational measures of hostile parenting and self-report measures of authoritarian parenting style uniquely predicted a larger ERN in children 3 years later. We previously reported that children in this sample with anxiety disorders were characterized by an increased ERN. A mediation analysis indicated that ERN magnitude mediated the relationship between harsh parenting and child anxiety disorder. Results suggest that parenting may shape children’s error processing through environmental conditioning and thereby risk for anxiety, although future work is needed to confirm this hypothesis.


Genes, Brain and Behavior | 2012

Additive effects of the dopamine D2 receptor and dopamine transporter genes on the error‐related negativity in young children

Alexandria Meyer; Daniel N. Klein; Dana C. Torpey; Autumn Kujawa; Elizabeth P. Hayden; Haroon I. Sheikh; Shiva M. Singh; Greg Hajcak

The error‐related negativity (ERN) is a negative deflection in the event‐related potential that occurs approximately 50ms following the commission of an error at fronto‐central electrode sites. Previous models suggest dopamine plays a role in the generation of the ERN. We recorded event‐related potentials (ERPs) while 279 children aged 5–7 years completed a simple Go/No‐Go task; the ERN was examined in relation to the dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2) and dopamine transporter (DAT1) genes. Results suggest an additive effect of the DRD2 and DAT1 genotype on ERN magnitude such that children with at least one DRD2 A1 allele and children with at least one DAT1 9 allele have an increased (i.e. more negative) ERN. These results provide further support for the involvement of dopamine in the generation of the ERN.


Development and Psychopathology | 2014

Emotion recognition in preschool children: Associations with maternal depression and early parenting

Autumn Kujawa; Lea R. Dougherty; C. Emily Durbin; Rebecca S. Laptook; Dana C. Torpey; Daniel N. Klein

Emotion knowledge in childhood has been shown to predict social functioning and psychological well-being, but relatively little is known about parental factors that influence its development in early childhood. There is some evidence that both parenting behavior and maternal depression are associated with emotion recognition, but previous research has only examined these factors independently. The current study assessed auditory and visual emotion recognition ability among a large sample of preschool children to examine typical emotion recognition skills in children of this age, as well as the independent and interactive effects of maternal and paternal depression and negative parenting (i.e., hostility and intrusiveness). Results indicated that children were most accurate at identifying happy emotional expressions. The lowest accuracy was observed for neutral expressions. A significant interaction was found between maternal depression and negative parenting behavior: children with a maternal history of depression were particularly sensitive to the negative effects of maladaptive parenting behavior on emotion recognition ability. No significant effects were found for paternal depression. These results highlight the importance of examining the effects of multiple interacting factors on childrens emotional development and provide suggestions for identifying children for targeted preventive interventions.


Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease | 2007

Retrospective reports of parenting in depressed adults with and without comorbid panic disorder and social anxiety disorder

Dana C. Torpey; Thomas M. Olino; Daniel N. Klein

Previous research has examined the role of parenting in the development of depression and anxiety disorders using retrospective reports of parenting behaviors. However, most studies have not considered comorbidity; the few that have did not differentially examine individual anxiety disorders and yielded inconsistent results. The present study compared retrospective parenting reports given by depressed individuals with no comorbid anxiety disorder, comorbid panic disorder, and comorbid social anxiety disorder. Results indicated that depressed men with panic disorder reported significantly greater maternal and nonsignificantly greater paternal protectiveness than depressed men without panic disorder but not than depressed women with and without panic disorder. No differences were found for the retrospective parenting reports given by depressed participants with or without social anxiety disorder. This work highlights the importance of examining specific anxiety disorders rather than grouping all depressed patients with any anxiety disorder together, as well as examining males and females separately when investigating the influence of parental behavior.


Current Psychiatry Reports | 2008

Chronic depression: Update on classification and treatment

Dana C. Torpey; Daniel N. Klein

Collaboration


Dive into the Dana C. Torpey's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Autumn Kujawa

Pennsylvania State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Greg Hajcak

Florida State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jiyon Kim

Stony Brook University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

C. Emily Durbin

Michigan State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Sara J. Bufferd

California State University San Marcos

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge