Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Deborah Lowe Vandell is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Deborah Lowe Vandell.


Child Development | 2010

Do Effects of Early Child Care Extend to Age 15 Years? Results From the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development

Deborah Lowe Vandell; Jay Belsky; Margaret Burchinal; Laurence Steinberg; Nathan Vandergrift

Relations between nonrelative child care (birth to 4(1/2) years) and functioning at age 15 were examined (N = 1,364). Both quality and quantity of child care were linked to adolescent functioning. Effects were similar in size as those observed at younger ages. Higher quality care predicted higher cognitive-academic achievement at age 15, with escalating positive effects at higher levels of quality. The association between quality and achievement was mediated, in part, by earlier child-care effects on achievement. High-quality early child care also predicted youth reports of less externalizing behavior. More hours of nonrelative care predicted greater risk taking and impulsivity at age 15, relations that were partially mediated by earlier child-care effects on externalizing behaviors.


Developmental Psychology | 1999

After-School Activities and the Development of Low-Income Urban Children: A Longitudinal Study.

Jill K. Posner; Deborah Lowe Vandell

After-school activities of 194 African American and White children from low-income households were studied from 3rd to 5th grade to determine relations with (a) child, family, and contextual variables and (b) childrens adjustment over time. Girls were more likely to engage in academic activities and socializing, whereas boys were more likely to play coached sports. Children who attended after-school programs spent more time on academic and extracurricular activities, whereas children in informal care settings spent more time watching TV and hanging out. Evidence of transactional relations between after-school activities and child adjustment was found. Time spent in activities between 3rd and 5th grades was related to childrens adjustment in 5th grade. In addition, child adjustment measured in 3rd grade was associated with time in different activities in 5th grade.


Early Childhood Research Quarterly | 2002

Do regulable features of child-care homes affect children’s development?

K. Alison Clarke-Stewart; Deborah Lowe Vandell; Margaret Burchinal; Marion O’Brien; Kathleen McCartney

Abstract Data from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care were used to assess whether regulable features of child-care homes affect children’s development. Child-care homes selected were those in which there were at least two children and the care provider received payment for child care (ns=164 when the study children were 15 months old, 172 at 24 months, and 146 at 36 months). Caregivers who were better educated and had received more recent and higher levels of training provided richer learning environments and warmer and more sensitive caregiving. Caregivers who had more child-centered beliefs about how to handle children also provided higher quality caregiving and more stimulating homes. In addition, when settings were in compliance with recommended age-weighted group size cut-offs, caregivers provided more positive caregiving. Quality of care was not related to caregivers’ age, experience, professionalism, or mental health, or to the number of children enrolled in the child-care home or whether the caregivers’ children were present. Children with more educated and trained caregivers performed better on tests of cognitive and language development. Children who received higher quality care, in homes that were more stimulating, with caregivers who were more attentive, responsive, and emotionally supportive, did better on tests of language and cognitive development and also were rated as being more cooperative. These findings make a case for regulating caregivers’ education and training and for requiring that child-care homes not exceed the recommended age-weighted group size.


Merrill-palmer Quarterly | 2004

Early Child Care: The Known and the Unknown

Deborah Lowe Vandell

Child care research during the last 20 years has made considerable progress in addressing questions about (a) the effects of child care quality, (b) the effects of amount and timing of early child care, and (c) the effects of different types of care such as centers, child care homes, and relative care. This commentary summarizes the converging research evidence with respect to each of these questions and then outlines some future directions for child care research.


Development and Psychopathology | 2004

Affect dysregulation in the mother-child relationship in the toddler years: Antecedents and consequences

Virginia D. Allhusen; Jay Belsky; Cathryn L. Booth; Robert H. Bradley; Celia A. Brownell; Margaret Burchinal; Susan B. Campbell; K. Alison Clarke-Stewart; Martha J. Cox; Sarah L. Friedman; Kathyrn Hirsh-Pasek; Aletha C. Huston; Elizabeth Jaeger; Jean F. Kelly; Bonnie Knoke; Nancy L. Marshall; Kathleen McCartney; Marion O'Brien; Margaret Tresch Owen; Chris Payne; Deborah Phillips; Robert C. Pianta; Wendy Wagner Robeson; Susan J. Spieker; Deborah Lowe Vandell; Marsha Weinraub

The purpose of this study was to examine child, maternal, and family antecedents of childrens early affect dysregulation within the mother-child relationship and later cognitive and socioemotional correlates of affect dysregulation. Childrens affect dysregulation at 24 and 36 months was defined in the context of mother-child interactions in semistructured play and toy cleanup. Dyads were classified as dysregulated at each age based on high negative affect. Affect dysregulation was associated with less maternal sensitivity and stimulation, more maternal depressive symptoms, and lower family income over the first 36 months of life. Children with early negative mood, lower Bayley Mental Development Index scores and insecure-avoidant (15 months) or insecure-resistant attachment classifications (36 months) were more likely to be in an affect-dysregulated group. Controlling for family and child variables, affect-dysregulated children had more problematic cognitive, social, and behavioral outcomes at 54 months, kindergarten, and first grade. The findings are discussed in terms of the early role played by parents in assisting children with affect regulation, the reciprocal nature of parent-child interactions, and the contribution of affect regulation to childrens later cognitive, social, and behavioral competence.


Child Development | 2013

Preschool Center Quality and School Readiness: Quality Effects and Variation by Demographic and Child Characteristics.

Tran D. Keys; George Farkas; Margaret Burchinal; Greg J. Duncan; Deborah Lowe Vandell; Weilin Li; Erik Ruzek; Carollee Howes

This article examines associations between observed quality in preschool center classrooms for approximately 6,250 three- to five-year-olds and their school readiness skills at kindergarten entry. Secondary analyses were conducted using data from four large-scale studies to estimate the effects of preschool center quality and interactions between quality and demographic characteristics and child entry skills and behaviors. Findings were summarized across studies using meta-analytic methods. Results indicate small, but statistically significant associations for preschool center quality main effects on language and mathematics outcomes with little evidence of moderation by demographic characteristics or child entry skills and behaviors. Preschool center quality was not reliably related to socioemotional outcomes. The authors discuss possible explanations for the small effect sizes and lack of differential effects.


Developmental Psychology | 2004

Does class size in first grade relate to children's academic and social performance or observed classroom processes?

Virginia D. Allhusen; Jay Belsky; Cathryn Booth-LaForce; Robert H. Bradley; Celia A. Brownell; Margaret Burchinal; Susan B. Campbell; K. Alison Clarke-Stewart; Martha J. Cox; Sarah L. Friedman; Kathryn Hirsh-Pasek; Renate Houts; Aletha C. Huston; Elizabeth Jaeger; Deborah J. Johnson; Jean F. Kelly; Bonnie Knoke; Nancy L. Marshall; Kathleen McCartney; Frederick J. Morrison; Marion O'Brien; Margaret Tresch Owen; Chris Payne; Deborah A. Phillips; Robert C. Pianta; Suzanne M. Randolph; Wendy Wagner Robeson; Susan J. Spieker; Deborah Lowe Vandell; Marsha Weinraub

This study evaluated the extent to which first-grade class size predicted child outcomes and observed classroom processes for 651 children (in separate classrooms). Analyses examined observed child-adult ratios and teacher-reported class sizes. Smaller classrooms showed higher quality instructional and emotional support, although children were somewhat less likely to be engaged. Teachers in smaller classes rated typical children in those classes as more socially skilled and as showing less externalizing behavior and reported more closeness toward them. Children in smaller classes performed better on literacy skills. Larger classrooms showed more group activities directed by the teacher, teachers and children interacted more often, and children were more often engaged. Lower class sizes were not of more benefit (or harm) as a function of the childs family income. First-grade class size in the range typical of present-day classrooms in the United States predicts classroom social and instructional processes as well as relative changes in social and literacy outcomes from kindergarten to first grade.


Developmental Psychology | 1991

Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth: Choices in After-School Care and Child Development.

Deborah Lowe Vandell; Janaki Ramanan

After-school care was examined for 390 3rd through 5th graders who were part of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth


Developmental Psychology | 2013

Timing of high-quality child care and cognitive, language, and preacademic development

Weilin Li; George Farkas; Greg J. Duncan; Margaret Burchinal; Deborah Lowe Vandell

The effects of high- versus low-quality child care during 2 developmental periods (infant-toddlerhood and preschool) were examined using data from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care. Propensity score matching was used to account for differences in families who used different combinations of child care quality during the 2 developmental periods. Findings indicated that cognitive, language, and preacademic skills prior to school entry were highest among children who experienced high-quality care in both the infant-toddler and preschool periods, somewhat lower among children who experienced high-quality child care during only 1 of these periods, and lowest among children who experienced low-quality care during both periods. Irrespective of the care received during infancy-toddlerhood, high-quality preschool care was related to better language and preacademic outcomes at the end of the preschool period; high-quality infant-toddler care, irrespective of preschool care, was related to better memory skills at the end of the preschool period.


Journal of Educational Psychology | 1996

School choice, family characteristics, and home-school relations : Contributors to school achievement ?

Lee Shumow; Deborah Lowe Vandell; Kyungseok Kang

Urban low-income 5th-graders participated in a school-choice study. Families utilizing choice schools (N = 73) were more likely to be African American, lower-income, and high-risk neighborhood residents than families whose children attended assigned schools (N = 100). Firm-responsive parenting, family togetherness, and family supportiveness also were linked positively to utilization of choice. Parent involvement in childrens schooling was higher in neighborhood schools. School choice positively predicted childrens mathematics achievement and school orientation. Parents who chose schools rated the teachers as practicing more parent-involvement strategies than parents of assigned students, but teachers reported equal practices. Parent ratings of school quality did not differ between conditions, nor did teachers or parents report better relationships in either condition.

Collaboration


Dive into the Deborah Lowe Vandell's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Margaret Burchinal

University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Margaret Tresch Owen

University of Texas at Dallas

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jay Belsky

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kim M. Pierce

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Sarah L. Friedman

National Institutes of Health

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Greg J. Duncan

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge