Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Margaret Tresch Owen is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Margaret Tresch Owen.


Psychological Science | 2015

The Contribution of Early Communication Quality to Low-Income Children’s Language Success

Kathy Hirsh-Pasek; Lauren B. Adamson; Roger Bakeman; Margaret Tresch Owen; Roberta Michnick Golinkoff; Amy Pace; Paula K. S. Yust; Katharine Suma

The disparity in the amount and quality of language that low-income children hear relative to their more-affluent peers is often referred to as the 30-million-word gap. Here, we expand the literature about this disparity by reporting the relative contributions of the quality of early parent-child communication and the quantity of language input in 60 low-income families. Including both successful and struggling language learners from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development, we noted wide variation in the quality of nonverbal and verbal interactions (symbol-infused joint engagement, routines and rituals, fluent and connected communication) at 24 months, which accounted for 27% of the variance in expressive language 1 year later. These indicators of quality were considerably more potent predictors of later language ability than was the quantity of mothers’ words during the interaction or sensitive parenting. Bridging the word gap requires attention to how caregivers and children establish a communication foundation within low-income families.


Developmental Psychology | 2010

Testing a series of causal propositions relating time in child care to children’s externalizing behavior

Kathleen McCartney; Margaret Burchinal; Alison Clarke-Stewart; Kristen L. Bub; Margaret Tresch Owen; Jay Belsky

Prior research has documented associations between hours in child care and childrens externalizing behavior. A series of longitudinal analyses were conducted to address 5 propositions, each testing the hypothesis that child care hours causes externalizing behavior. Data from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network (NICHD) Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development were used in this investigation because they include repeated measures of child care experiences, externalizing behavior, and family characteristics. There were 3 main findings. First, the evidence linking child care hours with externalizing behavior was equivocal in that results varied across model specifications. Second, the association between child care hours and externalizing behavior was not due to a child effect. Third, child care quality and proportion of time spent with a large group of peers moderated the effects of child care hours on externalizing behavior. The number of hours spent in child care was more strongly related to externalizing behavior when children were in low-quality child care and when children spent a greater proportion of time with a large group of peers. The magnitude of associations between child care hours and externalizing behavior was modest. Implications are that parents and policymakers must take into account that externalizing behavior is predicted from a constellation of variables in multiple contexts.


Development and Psychopathology | 2004

Affect dysregulation in the mother-child relationship in the toddler years: Antecedents and consequences

Virginia D. Allhusen; Jay Belsky; Cathryn L. Booth; Robert H. Bradley; Celia A. Brownell; Margaret Burchinal; Susan B. Campbell; K. Alison Clarke-Stewart; Martha J. Cox; Sarah L. Friedman; Kathyrn Hirsh-Pasek; Aletha C. Huston; Elizabeth Jaeger; Jean F. Kelly; Bonnie Knoke; Nancy L. Marshall; Kathleen McCartney; Marion O'Brien; Margaret Tresch Owen; Chris Payne; Deborah Phillips; Robert C. Pianta; Wendy Wagner Robeson; Susan J. Spieker; Deborah Lowe Vandell; Marsha Weinraub

The purpose of this study was to examine child, maternal, and family antecedents of childrens early affect dysregulation within the mother-child relationship and later cognitive and socioemotional correlates of affect dysregulation. Childrens affect dysregulation at 24 and 36 months was defined in the context of mother-child interactions in semistructured play and toy cleanup. Dyads were classified as dysregulated at each age based on high negative affect. Affect dysregulation was associated with less maternal sensitivity and stimulation, more maternal depressive symptoms, and lower family income over the first 36 months of life. Children with early negative mood, lower Bayley Mental Development Index scores and insecure-avoidant (15 months) or insecure-resistant attachment classifications (36 months) were more likely to be in an affect-dysregulated group. Controlling for family and child variables, affect-dysregulated children had more problematic cognitive, social, and behavioral outcomes at 54 months, kindergarten, and first grade. The findings are discussed in terms of the early role played by parents in assisting children with affect regulation, the reciprocal nature of parent-child interactions, and the contribution of affect regulation to childrens later cognitive, social, and behavioral competence.


Early Childhood Research Quarterly | 2000

Caregiver-mother partnership behavior and the quality of caregiver-child and mother-child interactions

Margaret Tresch Owen; Anne M. Ware; Bill Barfoot

Abstract Parent-caregiver communication, particularly concerning the behavior and experiences of the child, is a means of linking the home and child-care contexts of the child’s experience and enriching the caregiver’s and parent’s capacity to provide supportive and sensitive care of the child. The Parent-Caregiver Partnership Scale was administered to 53 mothers of 3-year-old children and to the children’s primary caregivers in child-care centers ( n = 20) and less formal child-care settings ( n = 33) to examine relations of mother-caregiver communication about the child to the quality of caregiver-child and mother-child interactions. More communication between mother and caregiver about the child as reported by both mothers and caregivers was significantly related to more sensitive and supportive caregiver-child interactions in child care, even after controlling for the mother’s and caregiver’s childrearing beliefs that were related to partnership behavior and the quality of child care. The quality of mother-child interaction was significantly associated with the mother’s communication with her child-care provider about her child. After controlling for maternal childrearing beliefs, mothers who engaged in more partnership behavior with their providers were more supportive and sensitive with their children.


Developmental Psychology | 2004

Does class size in first grade relate to children's academic and social performance or observed classroom processes?

Virginia D. Allhusen; Jay Belsky; Cathryn Booth-LaForce; Robert H. Bradley; Celia A. Brownell; Margaret Burchinal; Susan B. Campbell; K. Alison Clarke-Stewart; Martha J. Cox; Sarah L. Friedman; Kathryn Hirsh-Pasek; Renate Houts; Aletha C. Huston; Elizabeth Jaeger; Deborah J. Johnson; Jean F. Kelly; Bonnie Knoke; Nancy L. Marshall; Kathleen McCartney; Frederick J. Morrison; Marion O'Brien; Margaret Tresch Owen; Chris Payne; Deborah A. Phillips; Robert C. Pianta; Suzanne M. Randolph; Wendy Wagner Robeson; Susan J. Spieker; Deborah Lowe Vandell; Marsha Weinraub

This study evaluated the extent to which first-grade class size predicted child outcomes and observed classroom processes for 651 children (in separate classrooms). Analyses examined observed child-adult ratios and teacher-reported class sizes. Smaller classrooms showed higher quality instructional and emotional support, although children were somewhat less likely to be engaged. Teachers in smaller classes rated typical children in those classes as more socially skilled and as showing less externalizing behavior and reported more closeness toward them. Children in smaller classes performed better on literacy skills. Larger classrooms showed more group activities directed by the teacher, teachers and children interacted more often, and children were more often engaged. Lower class sizes were not of more benefit (or harm) as a function of the childs family income. First-grade class size in the range typical of present-day classrooms in the United States predicts classroom social and instructional processes as well as relative changes in social and literacy outcomes from kindergarten to first grade.


Child Development | 2009

Early family and child-care antecedents of awakening cortisol levels in adolescence

Elizabeth J. Susman; Kortnee Barnett-Walker; Cathryn Booth-LaForce; Margaret Tresch Owen; Jay Belsky; Robert H. Bradley; Renate Houts; Laurence Steinberg

This study examined early observed parenting and child-care experiences in relation to functioning of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis over the long term. Consistent with the attenuation hypothesis, individuals (n = 863) who experienced: (a) higher levels of maternal insensitivity and (b) more time in child-care centers in the first 3 years of life had lower awakening cortisol levels at age 15. Associations were small in magnitude. Nonetheless, results were (a) additive in that both higher levels of maternal insensitivity and more experience with center-based care uniquely (but not interactively) predicted lower awakening cortisol, (b) not accounted for by later caregiving experiences measured concurrently with awakening cortisol at age 15 or by early demographic variables, and (c) not moderated by sex or by difficult temperament.


Parenting: Science and Practice | 2003

Parenting and Children's Behavior at 36 Months: Equivalence Between African American and European American Mother - Child Dyads

Leanne Whiteside-Mansell; Robert H. Bradley; Margaret Tresch Owen; Suzanne M. Randolph; Ana Mari Cauce

Objective. This study examines the similarity of parenting and the associations between maternal behavior and child problem and prosocial behavior across two racial groups. Design. Using data from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care (SECC), analyses included an examination of the comparability of maternal and child behavior between African American (n = 123) and European American (n = 953) families using multiple-group mean and covariance structures analysis (MACS). Observational data from two mother - child activities and maternal report of child behavior were used to construct three parenting and four child latent constructs. Results. Comparable measures of parenting (Responsive, Harsh, and Intrusive), child problem behavior (Externalizing and Internalizing), and child prosocial behavior (Compliance and Expressiveness) were found using partial invariance methods across racial groups. The associations between harsh, intrusive and responsive parenting and child behaviors were found to be similar across racial groups. Conclusions. Generality in the organization of parenting and similarity in their association to child behavior obtain across racial groups.


Journal of Family Issues | 1985

Intergenerational Influences on the Parent-Infant Relationship in the Transition to Parenthood

Martha J. Cox; Margaret Tresch Owen; Jerry M. Lewis; Cynthia Riedel; Lynda SCALF-McIVER; Ana Suster

In this study, questions were addressed concerning the intergenerational transmission of parent-child relationships in couples going through the transition to parenthood. During the second trimester of their first pregnancy, couples provided information concerning experiences of parenting in their family of origin, then were reinterviewed and observed interacting with their infants at 3 months postpartum. It was expected that when these young adults had reported prenatally better parenting by their parents on specific parenting variables, they in turn would experience early parenthood more adaptively and would show better parenting with their own infants. The results provide support for this hypothesis.


BMC Public Health | 2012

Family income trajectory during childhood is associated with adiposity in adolescence: a latent class growth analysis

Darla E. Kendzor; Margaret O Brien Caughy; Margaret Tresch Owen

BackgroundChildhood socioeconomic disadvantage has been linked with obesity in cross-sectional research, although less is known about how changes in socioeconomic status influence the development of obesity. Researchers have hypothesized that upward socioeconomic mobility may attenuate the health effects of earlier socioeconomic disadvantage; while downward socioeconomic mobility might have a negative influence on health despite relative socioeconomic advantages at earlier stages. The purpose of the current study was to characterize trajectories of family income during childhood, and to evaluate the influence of these trajectories on adiposity at age 15.MethodsData were collected as part of the Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD) between 1991 and 2007 at 10 sites across the United States. A latent class growth analysis (LCGA) was conducted to identify trajectories of family income from birth to 15 years of age. Analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were conducted to determine whether measures of adiposity differed by trajectory, while controlling for relevant covariates.ResultsThe LCGA supported a 5-class trajectory model, which included two stable, one downward, and two upward trajectories. ANCOVAs indicated that BMI percentile, waist circumference, and skinfold thicknesses at age 15 differed significantly by trajectory, such that those who experienced downward mobility or stable low income had greater adiposity relative to the more advantaged trajectories. Conversely, upwardly mobile children and those with consistently adequate incomes had similar and more positive outcomes relative to the most disadvantaged trajectories.ConclusionsFindings suggest that promoting upward socioeconomic mobility among disadvantaged families may have a positive impact on obesity-related outcomes in adolescence.


Monographs of The Society for Research in Child Development | 2014

IV. STABILITY OF ATTACHMENT SECURITY FROM INFANCY TO LATE ADOLESCENCE

Ashley M. Groh; Cathryn Booth-LaForce; R. Chris Fraley; Margaret Tresch Owen; Martha J. Cox; Margaret Burchinal

The first section of this Monograph emphasized methodological issues concerning the AAI (George et al., 1984–1996). From this point forward, we turn our attention to continuity and change in attachment security over time, and the antecedents of variation in adult attachment more generally. According to attachment theory, individuals develop internalized representations of their early attachment-relevant experiences with primary caregivers. Such representations are argued to have their roots in the attachment relationships that individuals share with their primary caregivers in early childhood (Bowlby, 1969/1982; Main et al., 1985). Thus, an important question concerns the degree to which attachment variation is stable over time. Accordingly, we begin this section by quantifying the degree of stability in attachment security from infancy to late adolescence, leveraging three measures of mother-child attachment security acquired from the SECCYD in early childhood. In infancy, individual differences in attachment are often measured using the Strange Situation procedure (Ainsworth et al., 1978), a laboratory procedure designed to activate infants’ attachment behavioral system via a series of increasingly stressful separations from and subsequent reunions with their primary caregivers. In brief, infants are classified as secure in the Strange Situation procedure when their separation distress is effectively relieved upon their caregiver’s return. Infants are classified as insecure if they either ignore their primary caregiver upon reunion (i.e., insecure-avoidant) or simultaneously seek, yet resist, their caregiver upon reunion (i.e., insecureresistant). Finally, infants who exhibit a momentary breakdown of one of these organized strategies are classified as disorganized (Main & Solomon, 1990).

Collaboration


Dive into the Margaret Tresch Owen's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Martha J. Cox

University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jay Belsky

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Margaret Burchinal

University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge