Elias Besevegis
National and Kapodistrian University of Athens
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Elias Besevegis.
Journal of Personality | 2003
Charles F. Halverson; Valerie L. Havill; James E. Deal; Spencer R. Baker; James B. Victor; Vassilis Pavlopoulos; Elias Besevegis; Liu Wen
Based on over 50,000 parental descriptors of children gathered in eight different countries, we used a combination of focus group sorting of descriptors in each country and factor analyses of instruments developed in four of the countries (United States, China, Greece, and the Netherlands) to describe children ages 3 to 12 years to select items for an instrument that would work well across countries to access personality. Through many factor analyses of indigenous items in each country, a core set of 141 items was used in three of the countries, with over 3000 parents responding to our instruments in China, Greece, and the United States. Much cross-comparative research analysis has resulted in 15 robust midlevel scales that describe the structures of parental descriptors that are common to the three countries. The data on the English (U.S.) sample are presented in detail. Links to temperament and behavior problems are presented and discussed.
Journal of Personality | 2012
Jennifer L. Tackett; Helena R. Slobodskaya; Raymond A. Mar; James E. Deal; Charles F. Halverson; Spencer R. Baker; Vassilis Pavlopoulos; Elias Besevegis
Childhood personality is a rapidly growing area of investigation within individual differences research. One understudied topic is the universality of the hierarchical structure of childhood personality. In the present investigation, parents rated the personality characteristics of 3,751 children from 5 countries and 4 age groups. The hierarchical structure of childhood personality was examined for 1-, 2-, 3-, 4-, and 5-factor models across country (Canada, China, Greece, Russia, and the United States) and age group (3-5, 6-8, 9-11, and 12-14 years of age). Many similarities were noted across both country and age. The Five-Factor Model was salient beginning in early childhood (ages 3-5). Deviations across groups and from adult findings are noted, including the prominent role of antagonism in childhood personality and the high covariation between Conscientiousness and intellect. Future directions, including the need for more explicit attempts to merge temperament and personality models, are discussed.
Journal of Career Development | 2007
Ekaterini P. Argyropoulou; Despina Sidiropoulou-Dimakakou; Elias Besevegis
The main purpose of this study is to examine the dimensions of career indecision among a sample of Greek high school students (N = 848) and to classify the students of the sample in regard to their career decision status. A second objective is the investigation of the relationship between career decision status groups and generalized self-efficacy, coping strategies, and vocational interests. The results suggest that four factors contribute to career indecision: absence of structure, need for career guidance, diffusion of interests, and personal conflict. The clustering procedure indicates that the students of the sample could be classified in three career indecision cluster groups: decided, exploring possibilities, and undecided. These cluster groups differ in all four career indecision factors, generalized self-efficacy, coping strategies, and vocational interests. Results are discussed in terms of a career counseling framework.
European Journal of Developmental Psychology | 2010
Elias Besevegis; Evangelia P. Galanaki
The aim of this study was to examine coping with loneliness during early, middle, and late childhood, which is a highly neglected research issue. Childrens perceptions of coping with loneliness and coping with real loneliness experiences in the past were investigated. Interviews were conducted with 180 second, fourth, and sixth graders from Athens, Greece. Qualitative analyses of childrens responses were done on the basis of a recent conceptualization of coping with stress in childhood and adolescence (Skinner & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2007), which was supported by the data. Statistically significant age and gender differences were found. Results are discussed from the viewpoint of the two literatures—coping and loneliness—that this study attempted to bring together, and suggestions for future research are made.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology | 2013
Oliver Schlösser; Michael Frese; Anna-Maria Heintze; Musaed Al-Najjar; Thomas Arciszewski; Elias Besevegis; George D. Bishop; Mirilia Bonnes; Chris W. Clegg; Ewa Drozda-Senkowska; Mauricio Gaborit; Dayra Garzon; Tia G. B. Hansen; Irena Heszen; Márta Juhász; Mary A. Keating; Wustari L. H. Mangundjaya; Norma Binti Mansor; Jacqueline K. Mitchelson; Alejandra Ortiz-Reynoso; Janak Pandey; Ubolwanna Pavakanun; Vassilis Pavlopoulos; José M. Peiró; Kristina Potocnik; Maria Helena Restrepo-Espinosa; Norbert K. Semmer; Elizabeth R. Ventura; Matthew Whoolery; Kan Zhang
We validate, extend, and empirically and theoretically criticize the cultural dimension of humane orientation of the project GLOBE (Global Leadership and Organizational Behavior Effectiveness Research Program). Theoretically, humane orientation is not just a one-dimensionally positive concept about being caring, altruistic, and kind to others as discussed by Kabasakal and Bodur (2004), but there is also a certain ambivalence to this concept. We suggest differentiating humane orientation toward in-group members from humane orientation toward out-group members. A multicountry construct validation study used student samples from 25 countries that were either high or low in humane orientation (N = 876) and studied their relation to the traditional GLOBE scale and other cultural-level measures (agreeableness, religiosity, authoritarianism, and welfare state score). Findings revealed a strong correlation between humane orientation and agreeableness, welfare state score, and religiosity. Out-group humane orientation proved to be the more relevant subfacet of the original humane orientation construct, suggesting that future research on humane orientation should make use of this measure instead of the vague original scale. The ambivalent character of out-group humane orientation is displayed in its positive correlation to high authoritarianism. Patriotism was used as a control variable for noncritical acceptance of one’s society but did not change the correlations. Our findings are discussed as an example of how rigid expectations and a lack of tolerance for diversity may help explain the ambivalent nature of humane orientation.
Journal of Adolescence | 2014
Theo A. Klimstra; Wim Beyers; Elias Besevegis
European Journal of Personality | 1996
Frosso Motti-Stefanidi; Elias Besevegis; Nikos Giannitsas
Journal of Adolescence | 2018
Inge Seiffge-Krenke; Malte Persike; Elias Besevegis; Cecilia Chau; Neslihan Güney Karaman; Lyda Lannegrand-Willems; Katharzyna Lubiewska; Iffat Rohail
Archive | 2008
Elias Besevegis; Vassilis Pavlopoulos
Archive | 2010
Vassilis Pavlopoulos; Elias Besevegis; Katerina Georganti