Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Erika Nurmsoo is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Erika Nurmsoo.


Child Development | 2009

Children’s Trust in Previously Inaccurate Informants Who Were Well or Poorly Informed: When Past Errors Can Be Excused

Erika Nurmsoo; Elizabeth J. Robinson

Past research demonstrates that children learn from a previously accurate speaker rather than from a previously inaccurate one. This study shows that children do not necessarily treat a previously inaccurate speaker as unreliable. Rather, they appropriately excuse past inaccuracy arising from the speakers limited information access. Children (N= 67) aged 3, 4, and 5 years aimed to identify a hidden toy in collaboration with a puppet as informant. When the puppet had previously been inaccurate despite having full information, children tended to ignore what they were told and guess for themselves: They treated the puppet as unreliable in the longer term. However, children more frequently believed a currently well-informed puppet whose past inaccuracies arose legitimately from inadequate information access.


Psychological Science | 2008

Preschoolers' Perspective Taking in Word Learning Do They Blindly Follow Eye Gaze?

Erika Nurmsoo; Paul Bloom

When learning new words, do children use a speakers eye gaze because it reveals referential intent? We conducted two experiments that addressed this question. In Experiment 1, the experimenter left while two novel objects were placed where the child could see both, but the experimenter would be able to see only one. The experimenter returned, looked directly at the mutually visible object, and said either, “Theres the [novel word]!” or “Wheres the [novel word]?” Two- through 4-year-olds selected the target of the speakers gaze more often on there trials than on where trials, although only the older children identified the referent correctly at above-chance levels on trials of both types. In Experiment 2, the experimenter placed a novel object where only the child could see it and left while the second object was similarly hidden. When she returned and asked, “Wheres the [novel word]?” 2- through 4-year-olds chose the second object at above-chance levels. Preschoolers do not blindly follow gaze, but consider the linguistic and pragmatic context when learning a new word.


Developmental Science | 2012

Best Friends: Children Use Mutual Gaze to Identify Friendships in Others.

Erika Nurmsoo; Shiri Einav; Bruce M. Hood

This study examined childrens ability to use mutual eye gaze as a cue to friendships in others. In Experiment 1, following a discussion about friendship, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds were shown animations in which three cartoon children looked at one another, and were told that one target character had a best friend. Although all age groups accurately detected the mutual gaze between the target and another character, only 5- and 6-year-olds used this cue to infer friendship. Experiment 2 replicated the effect with 5- and 6-year-olds when the target character was not explicitly identified. Finally, in Experiment 3, where the attribution of friendship could only be based on synchronized mutual gaze, 6-year-olds made this attribution, while 4- and 5-year-olds did not. Children occasionally referred to mutual eye gaze when asked to justify their responses in Experiments 2 and 3, but it was only by the age of 6 that reference to these cues correlated with the use of mutual gaze in judgements of affiliation. Although younger children detected mutual gaze, it was not until 6 years of age that children reliably detected and justified mutual gaze as a cue to friendship.


Journal of Child Language | 2016

How children aged 2;6 tailor verbal expressions to interlocutor informational needs.

Kirsten Abbot-Smith; Erika Nurmsoo; Rebecca Croll; Heather J. Ferguson; Michael A. Forrester

Although preschoolers are pervasively underinformative in their actual usage of verbal reference, a number of studies have shown that they nonetheless demonstrate sensitivity to listener informational needs, at least when environmental cues to this are obvious. We investigated two issues. The first concerned the types of visual cues to interlocutor informational needs which children aged 2;6 can process whilst producing complex referring expressions. The second was whether performance in experimental tasks related to naturalistic conversational proficiency. We found that 2;6-year-olds used fewer complex expressions when the objects were dissimilar compared to highly similar objects, indicating that they tailor their verbal expressions to the informational needs of another person, even when the cue to the informational need is relatively opaque. We also found a correlation between conversational skills as rated by the parents and the degree to which 2;6-year-olds could learn from feedback to produce complex referring expressions.


Language | 2017

The role of timing and prototypical causality on how preschoolers fast-map novel verb meanings

Kirsten Abbot-Smith; Mutsumi Imai; Samantha Durrant; Erika Nurmsoo

In controlled contexts, young children find it more difficult to learn novel words for actions than words for objects: Imai et al. found that English-speaking three-year-olds mistakenly choose a novel object as a referent for a novel verb about 42% of the time despite hearing the verb in a transitive sentence. The current two studies investigated whether English three- and five-year-old children would find resultative actions easier (since they are prototypically causative) than the non-resultative, durative event types used in Imai et al.’s studies. The reverse was true. Furthermore, if the novel verbs were taught on completion of the action, this did not improve performance, which contrasts with previous findings. The resultative actions in the two studies reported here were punctual, change-of-location events which may be less visually salient than the non-resulative, durative actions. Visual salience may play a greater role than does degree of action causality in the relative ease of verb learning even at three years.


Developmental Science | 2009

Identifying unreliable informants: do children excuse past inaccuracy?

Erika Nurmsoo; Elizabeth J. Robinson


Cognitive Development | 2009

When Do Children Learn from Unreliable Speakers

Elizabeth J. Robinson; Erika Nurmsoo


Cognitive Development | 2008

Children's working understanding of knowledge sources: Confidence in knowledge gained from testimony

Elizabeth J. Robinson; S. N. Haigh; Erika Nurmsoo


British Journal of Development Psychology | 2011

Gaining knowledge via other minds: Children's flexible trust in others as sources of information

Elizabeth J. Robinson; Stephen A. Butterfill; Erika Nurmsoo


Review of Philosophy and Psychology | 2010

Children's selective learning from others

Erika Nurmsoo; Elizabeth J. Robinson; Stephen A. Butterfill

Collaboration


Dive into the Erika Nurmsoo's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Shiri Einav

Oxford Brookes University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge