Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Gabriel I. Cook is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Gabriel I. Cook.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2003

Interference to Ongoing Activities Covaries With the Characteristics of an Event-Based Intention

Richard L. Marsh; Jason L. Hicks; Gabriel I. Cook; Jeffrey S. Hansen; Andrew L. Pallos

Previous studies of event-based prospective memory have demonstrated that the character of an ongoing task can affect cue detection. By contrast, this study demonstrated that there is a reciprocal relationship insofar as cue-verification and response-retrieval processes interfered with making a response in the ongoing task. The amount of interference was determined by the type of intention, which was manipulated to affect the complexity of verification and retrospective response retrieval. These relationships were true even when the interference caused by cue detection was separated from a more general effect to ongoing-task performance caused by shifts in attentional allocation policies. The results have theoretical implications for models that attempt to specify the cognitive microstructure of event-based prospective memory.


Memory & Cognition | 2006

Task interference from prospective memories covaries with contextual associations of fulfilling them

Richard L. Marsh; Jason L. Hicks; Gabriel I. Cook

One of the current issues in the field of prospective memory concerns whether having an intention produces a cost to other ongoing activities (called task interference). The evidence to date suggests that certain intentions held over the shorter term do interfere with other tasks. Because the cumulative effect of such costs would be prohibitively expensive in everyday life, the present study examined one means by which that interference may be reduced. Participants who formed a specific association to fulfilling an intention in a future context did not exhibit task interference over the intervening period until that context was encountered. This outcome was observed with both an event-based and a time-based prospective memory task. The results suggest that associating intention fulfillment with a specific context can eliminate task interference, and they emphasize the importance of studying intentions that are linked to future contexts versus those that are not.


Memory & Cognition | 2007

Source monitoring is not always enhanced for valenced material

Gabriel I. Cook; Jason L. Hicks; Richard L. Marsh

Source monitoring for valenced materials has received very little attention from researchers interested in the residual effects that emotion can have on memory. The three previous studies that examined memory for valenced material found a source-monitoring enhancement effect. By contrast, we used two different combinations of sources and found a novel, consistent source-monitoring deficit for valenced words as compared with neutral ones. In addition, this memory deficit for contextual details did not consistently covary with item memory. We assert that it is possible to obtain an effect in which heightened attention toward valenced material reduces the binding of contextual details into memory.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2011

An Observation on the Spontaneous Noticing of Prospective Memory Event-Based Cues

Justin B. Knight; J. Thadeus Meeks; Richard L. Marsh; Gabriel I. Cook; Gene A. Brewer; Jason L. Hicks

In event-based prospective memory, current theories make differing predictions as to whether intention-related material can be spontaneously noticed (i.e., noticed without relying on preparatory attentional processes). In 2 experiments, participants formed an intention that was contextually associated to the final phase of the experiment, and lures that overlapped to differing degrees with the features of the intention-related cues were embedded in the initial phase. When participants were outside of the appropriate responding context (i.e., the initial phase), they exhibited slower latencies to lures that exactly matched the features of their intention compared with other types of lures and control words. In addition, on a final remember/know recognition test, participants reported having greater subjective recollection for the occurrence of the exact-match lures. These results suggest that exact-match lures were spontaneously noticed and differentially processed in the absence of any observable preparatory attentional processes. The findings have implications for the theoretical debate over whether preparatory attention must always be relied upon to notice intention-related material.


Memory & Cognition | 2006

Task interference from event-based intentions

Richard L. Marsh; Gabriel I. Cook; Jason L. Hicks

Task interference occurs in prospective memory tasks when an intention deleteriously affects performance on an ongoing activity in some way. Several studies have shown that task interference can manifest itself in slower latencies to perform an ongoing task. Recent evidence demonstrates that associating intentions to certain performance contexts affects prospective memory performance (see, e.g., Cook, Marsh, & Hicks, 2005). In the present study, an intention was associated with a particular stimulus class, such as pictures or words. We found that task interference could be reduced when participants could reliably predict that the material about to be processed was irrelevant to the intention. This material- specific interference effect was found on a trial-by-trial basis in a random sequence of two different kinds of materials across two experiments and with blocking manipulation in another experiment. These results demonstrate that task interference is not a monolithic construct; rather, it results from dynamic and flexible attentional allocation strategies that can change on a trial-by-trial basis.


Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition | 2007

Comparing older and younger adults in an event-based prospective memory paradigm containing an output monitoring component.

Richard L. Marsh; Jason L. Hicks; Gabriel I. Cook; Christopher B. Mayhorn

ABSTRACT Two experiments with younger and older adults were conducted to investigate the output-monitoring component of event-based prospective memory. In the standard form of the task, participants must remember to press a key when a certain class of items is encountered. To evaluate output monitoring, event-based cues were repeated and participants were asked to press a different key if they could remember that an earlier response was made to a particular cue. Younger adults forgot fewer of their successful responses, but displayed a distinct bias to claim that they had responded earlier when actually they had forgotten to respond. By contrast, older adults displayed this bias much less frequently. Elaborated responding to cues had the effect of improving the performance of younger, but not older adults. The results are discussed in terms of natural repetitions and omission errors that might be made in everyday prospective memory tasks.


Memory | 2006

Gender and orientation stereotypes bias source-monitoring attributions

Richard L. Marsh; Gabriel I. Cook; Jason L. Hicks

Four experiments were conducted to determine whether gender stereotypes influence source-monitoring decision processes. Statements that were consistent with a male were more often correctly attributed to a male source and less frequently correctly attributed to a female. The reverse was true for items traditionally associated with a female. Both of these biases were reversed if participants believed the speaker was either a gay male or a lesbian female. These effects persisted under divided attention during test, suggesting that they are caused by automatic influences. But these biases were partially attenuated when participants first considered the detrimental impact of stereotypes. Because these biases were absent for gender-neutral statements, the results from this study show that the content of a memory can influence judgements about the context in which something was learned. The authors argue that the data are most consistent with a heuristic, early selection process that can be influenced by a conscious, late correction process (e.g., Jacoby, Kelly, & McElree, 1999).


Psychology of Learning and Motivation | 2006

An Analysis of Prospective Memory

Richard L. Marsh; Gabriel I. Cook; Jason L. Hicks

Publisher Summary This chapter discusses prospective memory analysis and demonstrates that better integrating retrospective memory theories (and principles) with research on prospective memory can profitably advance understanding of prospective memory. Consequently, some similarities and functional relationships between retrospective and prospective memory phenomena that have previously been overlooked in the research reports on prospective memory have been elucidated. By making explicit the uniformities and analogous components of prospective and retrospective memory, it provides an overarching approach for generating new ideas for empirical work on prospective memory. How those cognitive processes that are well established in the retrospective memory literature might change the mental representation of prospective memories have been explained. The chapter analyzes how various intentions are formed in memory (encoding), consider next how they are realized (retrieved), and finally scrutinize how representations of intentions can change over the interval between formation and realization.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2006

Source memory in the absence of successful cued recall.

Gabriel I. Cook; Richard L. Marsh; Jason L. Hicks

Five experiments were conducted to address the question of whether source information could be accessed in the absence of being able to recall an item. The authors used a paired-associate learning paradigm in which cue-target word pairs were studied, and target recall was requested in the presence of the cue. When target recall failed, participants were asked to make a source judgment of whether a man or woman spoke the unrecalled item. In 3 of the 5 experiments, source accuracy was at or very close to chance. By contrast, if cue-target pairs were studied multiple times or participants knew in advance of learning that a predictive judgment would be required, then predictive source accuracy was well above chance. These data are suggestive that context information may not play a very large role in metacognitive judgments such as feeling-of-knowing ratings or putting one into a tip-of-the-tongue state without strong and specific encoding procedures. These same results also highlight the important role that item memory plays in retrieving information about the context in which an item was experienced.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2005

An Observation on the Role of Context Variability in Free Recall

Jason L. Hicks; Richard L. Marsh; Gabriel I. Cook

The authors conducted 3 experiments investigating the effect of context variability and word frequency on free recall. Context variability refers to the number of pre-experimental contexts in which a given word is experienced. Both between-subjects and within-subjects manipulations of context variability demonstrated a distinct advantage for low context variability words. Standard word frequency effects were obtained in 2 of the 3 experiments, but the common finding of no word frequency differences in mixed lists of high and low word frequency may depend on the level (low vs. high) of context variability. The authors speculate that the advantage for low context variability items may accrue from better item-to-list context associations or better storage of contextual information as a consequence of the smaller pre-experimental contextual fan that these items possess.

Collaboration


Dive into the Gabriel I. Cook's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jason L. Hicks

Louisiana State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

J. Thadeus Meeks

Southern Illinois University Edwardsville

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Gene A. Brewer

Arizona State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Adam R. Cobb

University of Texas at Austin

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge