Gabriel Ramos-Fernández
Instituto Politécnico Nacional
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Gabriel Ramos-Fernández.
Current Anthropology | 2008
Filippo Aureli; Colleen M. Schaffner; Christophe Boesch; Simon K. Bearder; Josep Call; Colin A. Chapman; Richard C. Connor; Anthony Di Fiore; R. I. M. Dunbar; S. Peter Henzi; Kay E. Holekamp; Amanda H. Korstjens; Robert Layton; Phyllis C. Lee; Julia Lehmann; Joseph H. Manson; Gabriel Ramos-Fernández; Karen B. Strier; Carel P. van Schaik
Renewed interest in fission‐fusion dynamics is due to the recognition that such dynamics may create unique challenges for social interaction and distinctive selective pressures acting on underlying communicative and cognitive abilities. New frameworks for integrating current knowledge on fission‐fusion dynamics emerge from a fundamental rethinking of the term “fission‐fusion” away from its current general use as a label for a particular modal type of social system (i.e., “fission‐fusion societies”). Specifically, because the degree of spatial and temporal cohesion of group members varies both within and across taxa, any social system can be described in terms of the extent to which it expresses fission‐fusion dynamics. This perspective has implications for socioecology, communication, cognitive demands, and human social evolution.
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology | 2004
Gabriel Ramos-Fernández; José L. Mateos; Octavio Miramontes; G. Cocho; Hernán Larralde; Bárbara Ayala-Orozco
Scale invariant patterns have been found in different biological systems, in many cases resembling what physicists have found in other, nonbiological systems. Here we describe the foraging patterns of free-ranging spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) in the forest of the Yucatan Peninsula, Mexico and find that these patterns closely resemble what physicists know as Lévy walks. First, the length of a trajectory’s constituent steps, or continuous moves in the same direction, is best described by a power-law distribution in which the frequency of ever larger steps decreases as a negative power function of their length. The rate of this decrease is very close to that predicted by a previous analytical Lévy walk model to be an optimal strategy to search for scarce resources distributed at random. Second, the frequency distribution of the duration of stops or waiting times also approximates to a power-law function. Finally, the mean square displacement during the monkeys’ first foraging trip increases more rapidly than would be expected from a random walk with constant step length, but within the range predicted for Lévy walks. In view of these results, we analyze the different exponents characterizing the trajectories described by females and males, and by monkeys on their own and when part of a subgroup. We discuss the origin of these patterns and their implications for the foraging ecology of spider monkeys.
arXiv: Populations and Evolution | 2006
Denis Boyer; Gabriel Ramos-Fernández; Octavio Miramontes; José L. Mateos; Germinal Cocho; Hernán Larralde; Humberto Ramos; Fernando Rojas
Scale-free foraging patterns are widespread among animals. These may be the outcome of an optimal searching strategy to find scarce, randomly distributed resources, but a less explored alternative is that this behaviour may result from the interaction of foraging animals with a particular distribution of resources. We introduce a simple foraging model where individual primates follow mental maps and choose their displacements according to a maximum efficiency criterion, in a spatially disordered environment containing many trees with a heterogeneous size distribution. We show that a particular tree-size frequency distribution induces non-Gaussian movement patterns with multiple spatial scales (Lévy walks). These results are consistent with field observations of tree-size variation and spider monkey (Ateles geoffroyi) foraging patterns. We discuss the consequences that our results may have for the patterns of seed dispersal by foraging primates.
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology | 2006
Gabriel Ramos-Fernández; Denis Boyer; Vian P. Gómez
Precisely how ecological factors influence animal social structure is far from clear. We explore this question using an agent-based model inspired by the fission–fusion society of spider monkeys (Ateles spp). Our model introduces a realistic, complex foraging environment composed of many resource patches with size varying as an inverse power law frequency distribution with exponent β. Foragers do not interact among them and start from random initial locations. They have either a complete or a partial knowledge of the environment and maximize the ratio between the size of the next visited patch and the distance traveled to it, ignoring previously visited patches. At intermediate values of β, when large patches are neither too scarce nor too abundant, foragers form groups (coincide at the same patch) with a similar size frequency distribution as the spider monkey’s subgroups. Fission–fusion events create a network of associations that contains weak bonds among foragers that meet only rarely and strong bonds among those that repeat associations more frequently than would be expected by chance. The latter form subnetworks with the highest number of bonds and a high clustering coefficient at intermediate values of β. The weak bonds enable the whole social network to percolate. Some of our results are similar to those found in long-term field studies of spider monkeys and other fission–fusion species. We conclude that hypotheses about the ecological causes of fission–fusion and the origin of complex social structures should consider the heterogeneity and complexity of the environment in which social animals live.
International Journal of Primatology | 2005
Christina J. Campbell; Filippo Aureli; Colin A. Chapman; Gabriel Ramos-Fernández; Kim Matthews; Sabrina E. Russo; Scott Suarez; Laura G. Vick
Spider monkeys (Ateles spp.) are well known for their highly arboreal lifestyle, spending much of their time in the highest levels of the canopy and rarely venturing to the ground. To investigate terrestriality by Ateles and to illuminate the conditions under which spider monkeys venture to the ground, we analyzed ad libitum data from 5 study sites, covering 2 species and 5 subspecies. Three of the sites are in Central/North America: Barro Colorado Island (BCI), Panama (Ateles geoffroyi panamensis), Santa Rosa National Park, Costa Rica (A. g. frontatus), and Punta Laguna, Mexico (A. g. yucatanensis). The 2 remaining sites are in South America: Cocha Cashu Biological Station, Perú (A. belzebuth chamek) and Yasuni National Park, Ecuador (A. b. belzebuth). Terrestrialism by Ateles at all sites is rare; however, it is more restricted at the 2 South American sites. In South America, ground use only occurred in the contexts of eating soil or rotten wood and visiting salt licks. In contrast at the 3 sites with Ateles geoffroyi it rarely occurred in a feeding context, but instead more frequently while drinking from streams during the dry season, by adult females escaping attack by adult males, and as part of a chase game. In addition, on BCI adult males were on the ground before attacking adult females. We discuss potential explanations, e.g., climate, species differences, predation pressure, for the differences between the Central/North and South American observations.
American Journal of Primatology | 2011
Lauren J. N. Brent; Julia Lehmann; Gabriel Ramos-Fernández
Advances over the last 15 years have made social network analysis (SNA) a powerful tool for the study of nonhuman primate social behavior. Although many SNA‐based techniques have been only very recently adopted in primatological research, others have been commonly used by primatologists for decades. The roots of SNA also stem from some of the same conceptual frameworks as the majority of nonhuman primate behavioral research. The rapid development of SNA in recent years has led to questions within the primatological community of where and how SNA fits within this field. We aim to address these questions by providing an overview of the historical relationship between SNA and the study of nonhuman primates. We begin with a brief history of the development of SNA, followed by a detailed description of the network‐based visualization techniques, analytical methods and conceptual frameworks which have been employed by primatologists since as early as the 1960s. We also introduce some of the latest advances to SNA, thereby demonstrating that this approach contains novel tools for the study of nonhuman primate social behavior which may be used to shed light on questions that cannot be addressed fully using more conventional methods. Am. J. Primatol. 73:720–730, 2011.
Archive | 2003
Gabriel Ramos-Fernández; Bárbara Ayala-Orozco
The Yucatan Peninsula is currently a mosaic of forest in various stages of secondary succession, mostly attributed to human-induced disturbances. No more than 50 years ago, approximately 86,000 km2 of the peninsula were covered by medium, semi-evergreen forest (Rzedowski, 1978). Today only a few unprotected fragments of this vegetation type remain larger than 1,000 km2, and deforestation continues at a rate of 8,000 km2 per year (Challenger, 1998). How does this disturbance affect spider monkey populations? Do spider monkeys modify their behavior in any way to survive in a fragmented habitat? If so, what is the minimum size of a forest fragment that can support a healthy population of spider monkeys? How can the remaining populations be protected?
Physica A-statistical Mechanics and Its Applications | 2004
Denis Boyer; Octavio Miramontes; Gabriel Ramos-Fernández; JoséLuis Mateos; G. Cocho
We discuss various features of the trajectories of spider monkeys looking for food in a tropical forest, as observed recently in an extensive in situ study. Some of the features observed can be interpreted as the result of social interactions. In addition, a simple model of deterministic walk in a random environment reproduces the observed angular correlations between successive steps, and in some cases, the emergence of Levy distributions for the length of the steps.
Ethology Ecology & Evolution | 2000
Gabriel Ramos-Fernández; A. Núñez-de la Mora; John C. Wingfield; Hugh Drummond
Two-chick broods of the blue-footed booby develop a dominance relationship during the junior chick’s first 2 weeks of life, when behaviour of the subordinate chick is conditioned through aggression by its 4-day-older sibling. We used natural and experimental situations to test the Challenge Hypothesis, which predicts a rise in testosterone to regulate aggression only in socially unstable situations, and to investigate the role of corticosterone in the development of submissive behaviour in subordinates. In unmanipulated broods during the first 12 days, we found no differences among singletons, dominants and subordinates in testosterone levels, and corticosterone levels did not differ significantly. In experimental pairings of singleton/dominant and singleton/subordinate, testosterone was not detected and corticosterone increased significantly in dominants and singletons, but not in subordinates. These results indicate that the Challenge Hypothesis does not apply to these young birds. Whether or not circulating corticosterone regulates aggression in chicks awaits further experimentation.
Proceedings of the Royal Society series B : biological sciences, 2016, Vol.283(1829), pp.20152830 [Peer Reviewed Journal] | 2016
David A. Puts; Alexander K. Hill; Drew H. Bailey; Robert S. Walker; Drew Rendall; John R. Wheatley; Lisa L. M. Welling; Khytam Dawood; Rodrigo A. Cárdenas; Robert P. Burriss; Nina G. Jablonski; Mark D. Shriver; Daniel J. Weiss; Adriano R. Lameira; Coren L. Apicella; Michael J. Owren; Claudia Barelli; Mary E. Glenn; Gabriel Ramos-Fernández
In many primates, including humans, the vocalizations of males and females differ dramatically, with male vocalizations and vocal anatomy often seeming to exaggerate apparent body size. These traits may be favoured by sexual selection because low-frequency male vocalizations intimidate rivals and/or attract females, but this hypothesis has not been systematically tested across primates, nor is it clear why competitors and potential mates should attend to vocalization frequencies. Here we show across anthropoids that sexual dimorphism in fundamental frequency (F0) increased during evolutionary transitions towards polygyny, and decreased during transitions towards monogamy. Surprisingly, humans exhibit greater F0 sexual dimorphism than any other ape. We also show that low-F0 vocalizations predict perceptions of mens dominance and attractiveness, and predict hormone profiles (low cortisol and high testosterone) related to immune function. These results suggest that low male F0 signals condition to competitors and mates, and evolved in male anthropoids in response to the intensity of mating competition.