Georges Canevet
Centre national de la recherche scientifique
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Georges Canevet.
Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 2005
Robert Teghtsoonian; Martha Teghtsoonian; Georges Canevet
A pure tone changing continuously in intensity shows sweep-induced fading (SIF) of loudness as intensity sweeps down and may show a lesser degree of sweep-induced enhancement (SIE) as intensity sweeps up (Canévet & Scharf, 1990); the former effect has been calleddecruitment, the latterupcruitment. An opposite effect—upsweeps being judged to show more loudness change than downsweeps—has been reported by Neuhoff (1998). These disparate results might stem from several procedural differences. We found that differences in the sweep’s duration and intensity level did not account for the disparity, nor did the presence of a steady tone preceding the sweep. In a second experiment, direct judgments of sweep size, such as those Neuhoff’s (1998) listeners made, were affected not only by sweep size itself, but also by the intensity at the end of the sweep. The latter effect was especially marked for upsweeps. Neuhoff’s (1998) proposed “bias for rising intensities” was found only with a method for judging sweep size that is more sensitive to end level than to sweep size.
Hearing Research | 2005
S. Roman; Georges Canevet; Patrick Marquis; Jean-Michel Triglia; Catherine Liégeois-Chauvel
This study investigated the ability of cochlear-implanted patients to discriminate tone bursts in free field using the electrophysiological recordings of mismatch negativity (MMN). Seven cochlear-implanted patients (CIP) and eight control subjects (CS) were tested. Event-related potentials were recorded from either 32 or 64 electrodes in response to binaural stimuli using a passive oddball paradigm. Two stimulus-contrast conditions were used to produce MMN: The standard-tone frequency was fixed at 1 kHz, and the deviant-tone frequency was set at 2 or 1.5 kHz. The results show that response waveforms (N1/P2) are similar in latency and amplitude for CS and CIP, suggesting that pure-tone detection is performed over the same time window in both groups. These waveforms are also similar in left- and right-implanted patients, suggesting that electric stimulation of the auditory nerve activates both hemispheres in profound, bilateral hearing loss. Pure-tone audiograms and word-discrimination scores were also measured in each subject in an anechoic room and their relations with MMN data were examined. Correlations were found between the latency of MMN for a 1.5 kHz deviant and the thresholds obtained for pure-tone detection and word discrimination. MMN appears as a possible complementary clinical tool to objectively assess auditory sensitivity in cochlear-implanted populations. However, further improvements are still necessary before it can be used as a standard clinical examination.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1990
Georges Canevet; Bertram Scharf
A sound at a low level is heard as much softer after having decreased continuously from higher levels than if presented after a period of silence at that same low level. Canévet [Acustica 61, 256-264 (1986)] demonstrated this phenomenon for a tone that (1) decreased from 65 to 20 dB in 180 s; he also presented a tone that (2) increased from 20 dB, or (3) was presented as pairs of bursts at various levels in random order. Below about 40 dB, loudness changed most rapidly in the decreasing condition so that, at 20 dB, the tone was judged ten times softer than in conditions (2) and (3). In the present experiments, magnitude estimation was used to examine the possible role of judgmental biases and adaptation in this rapid loudness decline, which we call decruitment. Results show that decruitment did not come about because subjects made many successive loudness judgments; loudness declined as much when a tone was judged only twice, at the beginning and end of its 180-s decrease. In contrast, interrupting the decreasing tone so that it was heard only at 70 dB and 160 s later at 30 dB greatly diminished the decruitment. Similarly, pairs of 500-ms tone bursts presented at successively lower levels instead of continously decreasing did not show decruitment, suggesting that sequential biases are irrelevant. The likely cause of decruitment is sensory adaptation.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1989
Theophile Angelini; Georges Canevet; Marc Foti; François Santon
An earpiece comprises a suction cup sensor, a preamplifier, a potentiometer for varying the gain of the amplifier, a pass band filter and electronic means enabling the user to adjust the central frequency and/or the width of the pass band of the filter. One application of the invention is the construction of self-contained and individualized telephone handsets or headphones which may be connected without any wire connection on any telephone set.
Nature | 1999
Georges Canevet; Bertram Scharf; Robert S. Schlauch; Martha Teghtsoonian; Robert Teghtsoonian
Neuhoff reported that “rising level tones... change (in loudness) more than falling level tones despite having the same actual change in level... indicating that direction of change is an important (and previously unaddressed) factor in the perception of dynamic loudness change”, and speculated that: “In a natural environment this over-estimation could provide a selective advantage, because rising intensity can signal movement of the source towards an organism.” Leaving aside the question of why it may not be as important for survival to detect the movement of a sound source away from an organism, we dispute the assertion that there is no prior evidence about the influence of direction of change on the degree of change in perceived loudness. This evidence does exist and shows, in contrast to the result reported by Neuhoff, that declining signal intensity covers a greater range of loudness than does rising signal intensity.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1984
Mary Florentine; Søren Buus; Bertram Scharf; Georges Canevet
The noise level at which native and non‐native listeners could repeat about 50% of very simple English sentences was measured with an adaptive procedure. The 14 non‐native listeners were primarily native French speaking and were 19 to 53 years old. Four native listeners were 16 to 53 years old. On each trial the listener repeated one sentence presented at 70 dB SPL via loudspeakers in an anechoic chamber. If the entire sentence was correct, the noise level was increased; otherwise it was decreased. the step size was 5 dB until the first reversal and 2 dB thereafter. The speech reception threshold, SRT, was defined as the average noise level of ten trials following the first reversal. Three SRTs were measured for each listener. All listeners could repeat eight practice sentences presented in the quiet with 100% accuracy. The average SRTs were 57 dB SPL for listeners with minimal experience in English, 62 dB SPL for listeners with moderate experience, and 68 dB SPL for listeners with extensive experience. N...
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 1981
Marie-Claire Botte; Georges Canevet; Bertram Scharf
The loudness of a continuous 1000-Hz tone at 60 dB SPL was measured in the presence of an intermittent tone in the contralateral ear. Over 70 observers participated in a series of eight experiments. The method of successive magnitude estimation showed that the intermittent tone causes the steady tone to diminish in loudness within 2 or 3 min by 40% to 60%. The amount of this induced loudness adaptation depends weakly upon the presentation rate, frequency, and level of the contralateral tone. Loudness reduction of the steady tone is coupled with loudness enhancement of the intermittent tone in the opposite ear. Induced loudness adaptation was also revealed by interaural and cross-modality matching. Induced loudness adaptation depends strongly on interaural interaction and is probably related to lateralization and interaural funneling of loudness. Adaptation induced by an intermittent tone stands in marked contrast to the near absence of loudness adaptation, except near threshold, when a continuous sound is presented alone.
Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 2000
Robert Teghtsoonian; Martha Teghtsoonian; Georges Canevet
When a tone or broad-band noise sweeps smoothly from a moderate intensity to a low one, the loudness at the end of the sweep is far less than what would be predicted from its intensity. The accelerated reduction in loudness, which was first reported by Canévet (1986) and confirmed in several later reports, has been calledloudness decruitment, and has been tentatively interpreted as the result of some form of adaptation. Since both simple and induced adaptation have distinctive temporal profiles, we undertook a series of studies in which we varied the duration of a tone whose intensity was continuously changing, to see whether the effect of duration on decruitment resembled the effects of duration on adaptation. We discovered that the magnitude of decruitment remained unaffected when the duration of the sweep was reduced far below the durations of 90 to 180 sec that have been used in previous studies. The same effect was observed for durations of around 20 sec, but it declined rapidly to a low level at the lowest duration of 1.0 sec. This temporal pattern is strikingly different from what has been reported for either simple or ipsilaterally induced adaptation, which suggests that neither form of adaptation can account for the entire effect. We also wanted to know whether an analogous phenomenon could exist for a sensory modality other than hearing. In the present study, observers were asked to judge the apparent size of a solid disk on a computer monitor; the disk increased or decreased continuously in area, or appeared as a series of separate areas, either in random order or in ordered progressions. We found that, as in the case of loudness, apparent size decreased more rapidly when the areas decreased continuously than would have been predicted from the actual areas themselves. We also found that some part of the accelerated shrinkage was due to a response bias in the observers’ judgments that stemmed from knowledge that every value in a continuously changing series is predictably smaller (or larger, for a growing series). Whether the remaining part of the effect is a sensory phenomenon is an important issue for future research.
British Journal of Audiology | 1983
Georges Canevet; Bertram Scharf; Marie-Claire Botte
In two recent articles, Hood and Wade (1982) and Weiler et al. (1981) have argued that the loudness of a steady tone does not appear to decline over time unless listeners are given a reference sound by which to judge loudness. The present experiments show, by the method of successive magnitude estimation, that listeners do not need a reference sound in order (1) to track accurately the decline in the loudness of a tone slowly decreasing from 60 to 40 dB SPL or (2) to track the loudness decline of a constant-intensity tone under special conditions that lead to adaptation. Since a reference sound is not needed by listeners to track a decline in loudness, and since Hood and Wade and Weiler et al. have found a decline with a reference but not without, it follows that adding a reference sound to a sustained sound must induce adaptation that otherwise does not occur. In support of that interpretation, the present measurements show that when subjects are told to ignore the same reference sound as used by Hood and Wade and by Weiler et al.--an increment every 30 s to a steady tone--loudness still declines. The bigger the increment (20 dB v. 5 dB) and the longer (5 s v. 1 s), the more loudness declines. Thus, loudness adaptation may be induced not only by a contralateral intermittent sound (Botte et al., 1982) but also by an ipsilateral intermittent sound. However, under normal listening conditions at levels above about 30 dB SL, loudness does not adapt.
Audiology | 1985
Georges Canevet; Bertram Scharf; Marie-Claire Botte
Simple loudness adaptation is the decrease in loudness that takes place when a continuous sound is presented alone for a period of time. Simple adaptation normally occurs only when a sound is soft to begin with, no more than 30 dB above threshold; except for some persons with a retrocochlear lesion, sounds above 30 dB SL do not diminish in loudness over time. However, adaptation can be induced in at least two ways: (1) A steady sound to one ear, presented together with an intermittent sound to the contralateral ear, decreases in loudness by 50-60% within 3 min. (2) An otherwise steady sound that is intermittently increased in level by at least 5 dB becomes softer during its weaker periods. When, for example, a 40-dB tone is increased every 20 s to 60 dB for 15 s, its loudness decreases by about 50% within 3 min. We report measurements of both simple and induced adaptation on 10 persons listening to a 1 000-Hz tone via earphones or from a loudspeaker. The results provide an overview of both types of adaptation. They also permitted a correlational analysis that reveals some of the similarities and differences between the two kinds of adaptation.