Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Gerrit Storms is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Gerrit Storms.


Behavior Research Methods | 2008

Word associations: Network and semantic properties

Simon De Deyne; Gerrit Storms

A number of properties of word associations, generated in a continuous task, were investigated. First, we investigated the correspondence of word class in association cues and responses. Nouns were the modal word class response, regardless of the word class of the cue, indicating a dominant paradigmatic response style. Next, the word association data were used to build an associative network to investigate the centrality of nodes. The study of node centrality showed that central nodes in the network tended to be highly frequent and acquired early. Small-world properties of the association network were investigated and compared with a large English association network (Steyvers & Tenenbaum, 2005). Networks based on a multiple association procedure showed small-world properties despite being denser than networks based on a discrete task. Finally, a semantic taxonomy was used to investigate the composition of semantic types in association responses. The majority of responses were thematically related situation responses and entity responses referring to parts, shape, or color. Since the association task required multiple responses per cue, the interaction between generation position and semantic role could be investigated and discussed in the framework of recent theories of natural concept representations (Barsalou, Santos, Simmons, & Wilson, in press).


Behavior Research Methods | 2008

Word associations: norms for 1,424 Dutch words in a continuous task.

Simon De Deyne; Gerrit Storms

This study describes the collection of a large set of word association norms. In a continuous word association task, norms for 1,424 Dutch words were gathered. For each cue, three association responses were obtained per participant. In total, an average of 268 responses were collected for each cue. We investigated the relationship with similar procedures, such as discrete association tasks and exemplar generation tasks. The results show that the use of a continuous task allows the study of weaker associations in comparison with a discrete task. The effects of the continuous tasks were investigated for set size and the availability characteristics of the responses, measured through word frequency, age of acquisition, and imageability. Finally, we compared our findings to those of a semantically constrained version of the association task in which participants generated responses within the domain of a semantic category. Results of this comparison are discussed. The Appendix cited in this article is available at www.psychonomic.org/archive.


Psychology and Psychotherapy-theory Research and Practice | 2002

The quality of the caregiving relationship in informal care for older adults with dementia and chronic psychiatric patients.

Nele Spruytte; Chantal Van Audenhove; Frans Lammertyn; Gerrit Storms

The present study focuses on the dyadic relationship between a family carer and a patient. Besides clarifying the quality of the caregiving relationship in two populations of chronically ill patients, this investigation examines whether patient characteristics, carer characteristics and network characteristics are predictive of relationship quality in dementia caregiving. Partners, children or children-in-law caring for a relative suffering from dementia (N = 144) and partners or parents of persons suffering from chronic mental illness (N = 77) were interviewed with a semi-structured questionnaire. The measurement of relationship quality is based on the literature of Expressed Emotion and covers two dimensions: the level of criticism and the level of warmth. In general, the relationship between a carer and his or her chronically ill relative was marked by a low level of conflict or criticism and a high degree of warmth. The main predictors of a poorer relationship quality were disturbances in the patients behaviour and the carers perception of these disturbances. Our results suggest that, rather than limiting investigations to the burden experienced by the family carer, future research and interventions on chronically ill patients should focus on the quality of the carer-patient relationship and its determinants.


Behavior Research Methods | 2008

Exemplar by feature applicability matrices and other Dutch normative data for semantic concepts

Simon De Deyne; Steven Verheyen; Eef Ameel; Wolf Vanpaemel; Matthew J. Dry; Wouter Voorspoels; Gerrit Storms

Features are at the core of many empirical and modeling endeavors in the study of semantic concepts. This article is concerned with the delineation of features that are important in natural language concepts and the use of these features in the study of semantic concept representation. The results of a feature generation task in which the exemplars and labels of 15 semantic categories served as cues are described. The importance of the generated features was assessed by tallying the frequency with which they were generated and by obtaining judgments of their relevance. The generated attributes also featured in extensive exemplar by feature applicability matrices covering the 15 different categories, as well as two large semantic domains (that of animals and artifacts). For all exemplars of the 15 semantic categories, typicality ratings, goodness ratings, goodness rank order, generation frequency, exemplar associative strength, category associative strength, estimated age of acquisition, word frequency, familiarity ratings, imageability ratings, and pairwise similarity ratings are described as well. By making these data easily available to other researchers in the field, we hope to provide ample opportunities for continued investigations into the nature of semantic concept representation. These data may be downloaded from the Psychonomic Society’s Archive of Norms, Stimuli, and Data, www.psychonomic.org/archive.


Psychonomic Bulletin & Review | 2008

In search of abstraction : The varying abstraction model of categorization

Wolf Vanpaemel; Gerrit Storms

A longstanding debate in the categorization literature concerns representational abstraction. Generally, when exemplar models, which assume no abstraction, have been contrasted with prototype models, which assume total abstraction, the former models have been found to be superior to the latter. Although these findings may rule out the idea that total abstraction takes place during category learning and instead suggest that no abstraction is involved, the idea of abstraction retains considerable intuitive appeal. In this article, we propose the varying abstraction model of categorization (VAM), which investigates the possibility that partial abstraction may play a role in category learning. We apply the VAM to four previously published data sets that have been used to argue that no abstraction is involved. Contrary to the previous findings, our results provide support for the idea that some form of partial abstraction can be used in people’s category representations.


Behavior Research Methods Instruments & Computers | 2004

Dutch norm data for 13 semantic categories and 338 exemplars

Wim Ruts; Simon De Deyne; Eef Ameel; Wolf Vanpaemel; Timothy Verbeemen; Gerrit Storms

A data set is described that includes eight variables gathered for 13 common superordinate natural language categories and a representative set of 338 exemplars in Dutch. The category set contains 6 animal categories (reptiles, amphibians, mammals, birds, fish, andinsects), 3 artifact categories (musical instruments, tools, andvehicles), 2 borderline artifact-natural-kind categories (vegetables andfruit), and 2 activity categories (sports andprofessions). In an exemplar and a feature generation task for the category nouns, frequency data were collected. For each of the 13 categories, a representative sample of 5–30 exemplars was selected. For all exemplars, feature generation frequencies, typicality ratings, pairwise similarity ratings, age-of-acquisition ratings, word frequencies, and word associations were gathered. Reliability estimates and some additional measures are presented. The full set of these norms is available in Excel format at the Psychonomic Society Web archive,www.psychonomic. org/archive/.


The Journal of Neuroscience | 2013

Similarity of fMRI Activity Patterns in Left Perirhinal Cortex Reflects Semantic Similarity between Words

Rose Bruffaerts; Patrick Dupont; Ronald Peeters; Simon De Deyne; Gerrit Storms; Rik Vandenberghe

How verbal and nonverbal visuoperceptual input connects to semantic knowledge is a core question in visual and cognitive neuroscience, with significant clinical ramifications. In an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiment we determined how cosine similarity between fMRI response patterns to concrete words and pictures reflects semantic clustering and semantic distances between the represented entities within a single category. Semantic clustering and semantic distances between 24 animate entities were derived from a concept-feature matrix based on feature generation by >1000 subjects. In the main fMRI study, 19 human subjects performed a property verification task with written words and pictures and a low-level control task. The univariate contrast between the semantic and the control task yielded extensive bilateral occipitotemporal activation from posterior cingulate to anteromedial temporal cortex. Entities belonging to a same semantic cluster elicited more similar fMRI activity patterns in left occipitotemporal cortex. When words and pictures were analyzed separately, the effect reached significance only for words. The semantic similarity effect for words was localized to left perirhinal cortex. According to a representational similarity analysis of left perirhinal responses, semantic distances between entities correlated inversely with cosine similarities between fMRI response patterns to written words. An independent replication study in 16 novel subjects confirmed these novel findings. Semantic similarity is reflected by similarity of functional topography at a fine-grained level in left perirhinal cortex. The word specificity excludes perceptually driven confounds as an explanation and is likely to be task dependent.


Psychonomic Bulletin & Review | 2002

Fruits and vegetables categorized: An application of the generalized context model

Tim Smits; Gerrit Storms; Yves Rosseel; Paul De Boeck

In the study reported in this paper, we investigated the categorization of well-known and novel food items in the categories fruits and vegetables. Predictions based on Nosofsky’s (1984, 1986) generalized context model (GCM), on a multiplicative-similarity prototype model, and on an instantiation model as applied in Storms, De Boeck, and Ruts (2001) were compared. Despite suggestions in the literature that prototype models predict categorization from large categories better than exemplar models do, our results showed that the exemplar-based GCM yielded clearly better predictions than did a (multiplicative-similarity) prototype model.


Acta Psychologica | 2014

Norms of age of acquisition and concreteness for 30,000 Dutch words

Marc Brysbaert; Michaël Stevens; Simon De Deyne; Wouter Voorspoels; Gerrit Storms

Word processing studies increasingly make use of regression analyses based on large numbers of stimuli (the so-called megastudy approach) rather than experimental designs based on small factorial designs. This requires the availability of word features for many words. Following similar studies in English, we present and validate ratings of age of acquisition and concreteness for 30,000 Dutch words. These include nearly all lemmas language researchers are likely to be interested in. The ratings are freely available for research purposes.


Psychonomic Bulletin & Review | 2008

Exemplars and prototypes in natural language concepts: A typicality-based evaluation

Wouter Voorspoels; Wolf Vanpaemel; Gerrit Storms

Are natural language categories represented by instances of the category or by a summary representation? We used an exemplar model and a prototype model, both derived within the framework of the generalized context model (Nosofsky, 1984, 1986), to predict typicality ratings for 12 superordinate natural language concepts. The models were fitted to typicality ratings averaged across participants and to the typicality judgments of individual participants. Both analyses yielded results in favor of the exemplar model. These results suggest that higher-level natural language concepts are represented by their subordinate members, rather than by a summary representation.

Collaboration


Dive into the Gerrit Storms's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Steven Verheyen

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Simon De Deyne

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Wouter Voorspoels

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Eef Ameel

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Wolf Vanpaemel

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Patrick Dupont

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Rik Vandenberghe

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ronald Peeters

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Rose Bruffaerts

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Chantal Van Audenhove

Université catholique de Louvain

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge